Abstract

Accelerated long-term forgetting (ALF) is the phenomenon whereby material is retained normally over short intervals (e.g. minutes) but forgotten abnormally rapidly over longer periods (days or weeks). ALF might be an early marker of cognitive decline, but little is known about its relationships with preclinical Alzheimer’s disease pathology and how memory selectivity might influence which material is forgotten.

We assessed ALF in ‘Insight 46’, a sub-study of the MRC National Survey of Health and Development (a population-based cohort born during the same week in 1946) (n = 429; 47% female; assessed at age ∼73 years). ALF assessment comprised visual and verbal memory tests: complex figure drawing and the face–name associative memory exam (FNAME). ALF scores were calculated as the percentage of material retained after 7 days, relative to 30 min. In 306 cognitively normal participants, we investigated effects on ALF of β-amyloid pathology (quantified using 18F-Florbetapir-PET, classified as positive/negative) and whole-brain and hippocampal atrophy rate (quantified from serial T1-MRI over ∼2.4 years preceding the ALF assessment), in addition to interactions between these pathologies. We categorized complex figure drawing items as ‘outline’ or ‘detail’, to test our hypothesis that forgetting the outline of the structure would be more sensitive to the effect of brain pathologies. We also investigated associations between ALF and subjective cognitive decline, measured with the MyCog questionnaire.

Complex figure ‘outline’ items were better retained than ‘detail’ items (mean retention over 7 days = 94% versus 72%). Amyloid-positive participants showed greater forgetting of the complex figure outline compared with amyloid-negative participants (90% versus 95%; P < 0.01). There were interactions between amyloid pathology and cerebral atrophy, such that whole-brain and hippocampal atrophy predicted greater ALF on complex figure drawing among amyloid-positive participants only [e.g. 1.9 percentage-points lower retention per ml/year of whole-brain atrophy (95% confidence intervals 0.5, 3.7); P < 0.05]. Greater ALF on FNAME was associated with increased rate of hippocampal atrophy. ALF on complex figure drawing was also correlated with subjective cognitive decline [−0.45 percentage-points per MyCog point (−0.85, −0.05); P < 0.05].

These results provide evidence of associations between some measures of ALF and biomarkers of brain pathologies and subjective cognitive decline in cognitively normal older adults. On complex figure drawing, ‘outline’ items were better remembered than ‘detail’ items, illustrating the strategic role of memory selectivity, but ‘outline’ items were also relatively more vulnerable to ALF in individuals with amyloid pathology. Overall, our findings suggest that ALF might be a sensitive marker of cognitive changes in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease.

Introduction

Accelerated long-term forgetting (ALF) is the phenomenon whereby information is retained normally over short intervals (minutes) but forgotten abnormally rapidly over longer periods (days or weeks).1 Although the mechanistic basis (whether ALF represents an impairment of memory consolidation or a failure of retrieval processes) and neurobiological underpinnings of ALF are poorly understood,2,3 the phenomenon highlights a critical phase of normal memory processing that is almost entirely ignored in routine clinical assessment. ALF has been predominantly described in people with epilepsy,4 including children,5 most commonly resulting from seizures affecting the temporal lobes, particularly in transient epileptic amnesia.6 It has also been described after minor stroke or transient ischaemic attack7 and childhood traumatic brain injury.8 Recently, there has been increasing interest in whether ALF could be a useful early clinical marker of memory impairments in Alzheimer’s disease (Ad); impairments that would be missed by standard memory tests, which typically assess recall over short delays (often ≤30 min). Three recent literature reviews on this topic9-11 present a mixed and nuanced picture of results, but suggest that ALF appears to be an early feature of cognitive decline in the preclinical and prodromal (mild cognitive impairment) stages of Ad.

The concept of preclinical Ad has evolved considerably in recent years and is now applied to cognitively unimpaired individuals with biomarker evidence of Ad pathology: amyloid and tau (usually identified from PET neuroimaging and/or CSF) and neurodegeneration (primarily identified from volumetric structural MRI).12 These pathological accumulations are detectable years, perhaps decades, before the onset of symptoms.13,14 However, studies investigating ALF in the preclinical stage of Ad have often lacked biomarker evidence of pathology and have instead been based on risk factors including age, APOE-ε4 genotype (the strongest genetic risk factor for Ad),15 family history of dementia and subjective cognitive decline (i.e. self-reported decline in cognition despite normal performance on objective cognitive tests).16 Evidence from these studies suggests the presence of ALF in APOE-ε4 carriers17,18 and in individuals with subjective cognitive decline.19-21 One study of healthy older adults found that 4-week delayed verbal memory recall was better than standard memory tests at predicting cognitive decline after 12 months.22 Other relevant studies have detected ALF in cohorts carrying rare genetic mutations causing autosomal dominant Ad, but who have not yet developed symptoms,23-25 with one concluding that ALF might be detectable up to a decade before symptom onset, earlier than other neuropsychological tests.25 To our knowledge, only one study has investigated associations between ALF and a biomarker of Ad pathology, reporting greater forgetting rates over 1 week in individuals with abnormal levels of CSF amyloid-β42.20 However, this group contained only 10 individuals, who also had subjective cognitive decline and performed more poorly than their amyloid-negative counterparts on the initial learning and recall tests, suggesting that their memory impairment was not specifically ALF.

There is therefore a need for evidence regarding whether ALF might be a sensitive marker of early subtle cognitive deficits in individuals with biomarker evidence of Ad pathology. This question is especially important in this new era of approved disease-modifying treatments, because sensitive cognitive measures are required for diagnosis and tracking of patients and for use as screening and outcome measures for clinical trials that will increasingly focus on preclinical populations, including those with subjective cognitive decline. Furthermore, through the identification of ALF in specific populations and a careful consideration of what is and is not recalled on a specific cognitive test (e.g. overall gist versus peripheral detail),26 especially with tasks used to detect ALF often differing markedly between studies,3,10 we might not only improve the sensitivity of these tests but also gain insight into what drives this phenomenon.

This study assessed ALF in the Insight 46 sub-study27 of the MRC National Survey of Health and Development (NSHD; the British 1946 birth cohort), a population-based cohort of individuals who were all born during the same week in March 1946. We investigated relationships between ALF and biomarkers of brain pathology in cognitively normal individuals, addressing the following questions: (i) Do participants with elevated amyloid show evidence of ALF? (ii) Is ALF associated with neurodegeneration, as indexed by whole-brain and hippocampal atrophy rates? and (iii) Is ALF correlated with subjective cognitive decline? Specific hypotheses addressing these questions are set out in the ‘Materials and methods’ section.

Materials and methods

The NSHD is the world’s longest continuously running birth cohort, with 25 data collections across childhood and adulthood.28 For the Insight 46 sub-study,27 502 NSHD participants completed a baseline assessment at University College London (UCL) between 2015 and 2018, at age ∼70 years. Four hundred and forty-two of these participants completed follow-up assessment at age ∼73 years. As detailed below and in Supplementary Fig. 1, this study focuses on cognitively normal participants. Detailed recruitment procedures and protocols have been described previously.27,29 At both baseline and follow-up, measures included cognitive tests, clinical history and examination, β-amyloid-PET imaging, brain MRI, and other biomarker (blood; CSF) and genetic measures as detailed elsewhere.27,30-32 The study was approved by the Queen Square Research Ethics Committee, London (reference 14/LO/1173). All participants provided written informed consent according to the Declaration of Helsinki.

The cognitive battery comprised paper-and-pencil tests and more novel computerized tasks, detailed in previous publications where we have reported cross-sectional associations between baseline cognition and biomarkers of brain health.32-35 At follow-up, the cognitive battery was complemented with the addition of an accelerated long-term forgetting (ALF) assessment, hence only cross-sectional data are currently available for this test.

Accelerated long-term forgetting assessment

Our ALF assessment used two memory tests: the 12-item face–name associative memory exam (FNAME-12),36 which requires learning and recall of names and occupations associated with pictures of faces, and complex figure drawing from the adult memory and information processing battery (AMIPB).37 The rationale for not including the list learning task used in our original study of ALF in presymptomatic dominantly inherited AD23 was to avoid interference and potential contamination with an existing 15-item list learning test embedded in the longitudinal NSHD assessments at ages 43, 53, 62 and 69 years.

Both tests included immediate recall and 30-min delayed recall, in addition to 7-day delayed recall and recognition captured using a telephone assessment (Fig. 1). This complies with guidance from Elliott et al.3 that assessments of ALF should use both verbal and non-verbal material, with testing of recall and recognition.

Assessment of accelerated long-term forgetting.
Figure 1

Assessment of accelerated long-term forgetting.

On the day of their assessment at the clinic, participants were given a sealed envelope containing the paper stimuli needed for the telephone call to take place 7 days later; they were instructed not to open this envelope in advance of the call. They were not warned to expect any cognitive tests; they were simply told that the purpose of the call was to follow up on some aspects of the clinical visit. Within the envelope was a set of further sealed envelopes, each containing material for one task, meaning that each task could be completed sequentially according to the instructions of the assessor. Owing to restrictions during the coronavirus pandemic, 22 participants were assessed via video call instead of in-person assessment, hence their testing procedure deviated slightly from the descriptions below, in that the stimuli for the main assessment and the 7-day assessment were presented to them via screenshare.

Face–name test

FNAME-1236 is a paired associative memory test, which was chosen because it meets the criteria of Elliot et al.3 and was already part of the existing cognitive battery, hence it could be adapted into an ALF assessment without adding to the length of the main battery or conflicting with existing memory tasks. We used FNAME-12 version A (version B is also available). The procedure (summarized in Fig. 1) was as follows. Participants received two exposures to 12 faces, each with a name and occupation (e.g. Nancy, Doctor). The faces were presented one by one, for 8 s each, on a computer screen. Each exposure was followed by an immediate recall test, in which participants were shown the 12 faces one by one and asked to state the name and occupation of each one. A third recall trial was administered after a delay of ∼10 min. After a ∼30-min delay, participants were shown 12 sets of three faces and asked to identify each previously learned face from the two distractors (facial recognition) and to state the name and occupation (the fourth recall test). They were then asked to select the correct name and occupation from three options comprising the correct answer, a distractor (a name or occupation that belongs with a different face in the set) and a name or occupation that did not feature in the set. During the 7-day telephone call, we repeated the same procedure as at the 30-min delay, with the stimuli presented on printed worksheets provided in the sealed envelopes. For each of the five recall trials (immediate 1, immediate 2, 10-min delay, 30-min delay and 7-day delay), the score is the number of names and occupations correctly recalled (maximum of 24). For each of the two facial recognition tests (30-min delay and 7-day delay), the maximum score is 12. For each of the two name and occupation recognition tests (30-min delay and 7-day delay), the maximum score is 24.

Complex figure drawing

Complex figure drawing was chosen based on its sensitivity to ALF in presymptomatic individuals carrying mutations causing familial AD23 and because it did not conflict with any existing memory tests in the battery. Complex figure drawing tests are widely used as measures of visual memory.38 We used AMIPB complex figure version A,37,39 which was presented to participants on a laminated card. Initially, participants were asked to copy the figure while it was directly in front of them, without a time limit. They were then asked to draw it from memory immediately after it had been removed from sight, and again after a delay of ∼30 min. During the 7-day telephone call, participants were asked to make a further drawing from memory, which they were then instructed to seal inside an envelope. To test recognition memory, they were subsequently shown four sets of three similar illustrations (on printed worksheets provided in the sealed envelopes) and were asked to identify which images exactly matched part of the original figure (with a maximum score of four). The four figure drawings (copy, immediate recall, 30-min recall and 7-day recall) were scored according to the AMIBP manual, which breaks the figure down into 36 items (lines or features). Most of the items are worth up to two points each, but four of the more complicated features are worth up to four points each, giving a maximum total score of 80. Points are deducted for items that are missing or are inaccurate (e.g. wrong size, wrong orientation, wrong position). Five raters carried out the scoring, meeting regularly to discuss queries and ensure consistent application of the scoring guidelines. We created a spreadsheet to record item-level scores (Supplementary material). All raters were blind to the amyloid status of participants.

When scoring the drawings, we noticed that the outline of the figure (i.e. the general box-like structure) tended to be preserved, whereas the internal details (e.g. internal lines and small features) were more likely to be forgotten (Fig. 2A). We also noticed that for some participants there appeared to be a qualitative shift after 7 days whereby the outline was no longer well reproduced (see Fig. 3 for some individual examples). This shift was apparent both from visual examination of the drawings and from the particular challenges of applying the scoring guidelines to some of the 7-day drawings. The scoring procedure requires raters to decide whether each item is present or absent and (if present) whether it is accurate or distorted in some way (e.g. too big, rotated, embellished). The manual advises raters to start by identifying a ‘reference set’ (a part of the drawing that can be treated as an anchor for other items to be judged in relationship to); for most drawings, this was the outline of the four boxes (Fig. 2A). For some 7-day drawings, the scoring was difficult and time-consuming, because there was no clear ‘reference set’ and the lines on the page did not have an obvious or unambiguous mapping onto the original diagram. This experience, along with seeing the outline stand out so distinctively in Fig. 2A (an earlier version of which we produced as an interim analysis for a conference poster)40 prompted our idea that there might be distinct processes underlying recall of the outline and recall of the detail after an extended (7-day) delay. We saw a parallel here with the premise of ALF, namely that the processes of memory storage and recall are somehow functioning differently over a long delay compared with a short delay.

‘Outline’ and ‘detail’ items in the complex figure from the adult memory and information processing battery (AMIBP). (A) Heat map, with each item coloured according to its mean score across the immediate, 30-min and 7-day recall trials. The heat map colours range from blue (representing the minimum mean score of 0.456) to green (representing the maximum mean score of 0.977). (B) Each item is numbered according to the scoring manual, with items that we have categorized as ‘outline’ highlighted in blue. (C) The bar charts show the frequency of scores awarded for each item in the full sample of Insight 46 participants (n = 429), with the scores represented as proportions (i.e. one is the maximum possible score). Numbers on the x-axis refer to the item numbers according to the scoring manual (see B). Bars are ordered according to the frequency of full-mark scores, i.e. starting with the best-remembered item. The blue rectangular box shows the items that participants consistently scored higher on, which we have categorized as ‘outline’ items, with the remaining items categorized as ‘detail’ items (as explained in the ‘Materials and methods’ section).
Figure 2

‘Outline’ and ‘detail’ items in the complex figure from the adult memory and information processing battery (AMIBP). (A) Heat map, with each item coloured according to its mean score across the immediate, 30-min and 7-day recall trials. The heat map colours range from blue (representing the minimum mean score of 0.456) to green (representing the maximum mean score of 0.977). (B) Each item is numbered according to the scoring manual, with items that we have categorized as ‘outline’ highlighted in blue. (C) The bar charts show the frequency of scores awarded for each item in the full sample of Insight 46 participants (n = 429), with the scores represented as proportions (i.e. one is the maximum possible score). Numbers on the x-axis refer to the item numbers according to the scoring manual (see B). Bars are ordered according to the frequency of full-mark scores, i.e. starting with the best-remembered item. The blue rectangular box shows the items that participants consistently scored higher on, which we have categorized as ‘outline’ items, with the remaining items categorized as ‘detail’ items (as explained in the ‘Materials and methods’ section).

Examples of responses to the complex figure drawing test. Responses from six participants are shown (each participant in a different column). The original drawings were made using pencil and paper. We wrote a python script to extract the pencil lines from scanned copies of the worksheets and colour them digitally. The choice of colours is arbitrary, but the colour intensity corresponds to the score received, with paler hues indicating lower scores.
Figure 3

Examples of responses to the complex figure drawing test. Responses from six participants are shown (each participant in a different column). The original drawings were made using pencil and paper. We wrote a python script to extract the pencil lines from scanned copies of the worksheets and colour them digitally. The choice of colours is arbitrary, but the colour intensity corresponds to the score received, with paler hues indicating lower scores.

Based on this, we hypothesized that such a breakdown in memory for the outline might reflect a more problematic (and potentially pathological) type of forgetting, potentially reflecting the cognitive consequences of preclinical Ad pathologies. To test this hypothesis, we created separate ‘outline’ and ‘detail’ scores, by categorizing each of the 36 items as either ‘outline’ or ‘detail’. We first converted the scores for each item to a proportion (between zero and one), to allow all the items to be compared against each other (Fig. 2C). Visually examining these bar charts (Fig. 2C), we perceived a step decrease in the frequency of full-mark responses after the highest-scoring 11 items, and we noticed that these 11 items were consistent across all three delay trials (despite minor variations in their ordering). These 11 items correspond to the outline of the complex figure (Fig. 2B). Thus, we categorized these 11 items as ‘outline’, creating a score out of 22 (given that all of these items were worth up to two points each). The remaining 25 items were categorized as ‘detail’, creating a score out of 58 (given that 21 items were worth up to two points and four items were worth up to four points).

ALF scores

Using a method derived from previous studies,3,23 ALF scores were calculated as the percentage of material recalled after 7 days, relative to the proportion recalled after 30 min (i.e. 7-day score/30-min score, multiplied by 100). This gives a ‘percentage retention’ score, which is preferable to simply using the 7-day recall score, because that does not capture forgetting itself.3 Previous studies have identified testing at 30 min and 7 days to be sufficient for identifying ALF.23,41 The complex figure drawing had three ALF scores reflecting overall retention of the diagram, retention of the ‘outline’ and retention of the ‘detail’ (Drawingtotal, Drawingoutline and Drawingdetail). The face–name test had a single ALF score based on total names and occupations correctly recalled, hereafter referred to as ALF-FNAME.

Of the 442 participants in the follow-up assessments, ALF scores were available for 429 participants. Reasons for missing data are detailed in Supplementary Fig. 1.

Biomarker measures

At both baseline and follow-up, participants underwent simultaneous β-amyloid-PET and multimodal MRI during a 60-min scanning session on a single Biograph mMR 3 T PET/MRI scanner (Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany), with intravenous injection of 370 MBq of the β-amyloid PET ligand, 18F-Florbetapir (Amyvid). Detailed imaging protocols have been described elsewhere.27,31

β-Amyloid deposition was quantified using the standardized uptake value ratio (SUVR) calculated from a composite of cortical regions of interest with a reference region of whole cerebellum, with partial volume correction. A cut-point for amyloid status (positive/negative) was determined using a mixture model to define two Gaussians and taking the 99th percentile of the lower (amyloid-negative) Gaussian at SUVR > 1.032.42 In all analyses involving SUVR, we have used the follow-up value, because this was contemporaneous to the ALF assessment. In all analyses involving amyloid status, we have used the follow-up data for the same reason, but for three participants with missing follow-up PET data, we have substituted baseline status (positive, n = 1; negative, n = 2). This maximizes the sample size and is based on our finding that few participants changed amyloid status between baseline and follow-up: of 269 amyloid-negatives at baseline, only 21 (7.8%) converted to amyloid-positive at follow-up.

Changes in whole-brain and hippocampal volumes between baseline and follow-up were quantified from T1-weighted MRI using the boundary shift integral.43 The boundary shift integral allows for comparison of brain volumes on serial imaging through a process of semi-automated brain segmentation and edge detection of the boundaries between brain and CSF.44 Annualized whole-brain and hippocampal atrophy rates were calculated by dividing the boundary shift integral by the interval between the two scans. Negative boundary shift integral values represent volume loss (atrophy).

Global white matter hyperintensity volume (a marker of cerebral small vessel disease that is common in older people and is associated with poorer cognition)32,34,45 was quantified from T1 and FLAIR images using an automated segmentation algorithm (Bayesian model selection), followed by visual quality control.46 Total intracranial volume (TIV) was generated using statistical parametric mapping software (SPM12; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm).47

APOE genotype was classified as ε4 carriers or non-carriers.27

Subjective cognitive decline questionnaire

Subjective cognitive decline at baseline and follow-up was measured using the MyCog questionnaire, which is part of the SCD-Questionnaire (SCD-Q).48 MyCog comprises 24 ‘yes/no’ questions about instrumental activities of daily living and assesses perceived decline over the preceding 2 years (e.g. ‘I find it harder to follow the plot of a book’). Scores range between 0 and 24, with higher scores indicating greater perceived cognitive decline.

We have previously reported cross-sectional results from our baseline assessments, showing that MyCog scores were correlated with trait anxiety scores,49 measured using the state and trait anxiety inventory (STAI).50 Our proposed explanation for this is that MyCog (like other measures of subjective cognitive decline) captures some general aspects of anxiety traits, such as a low estimation of one’s own abilities. Now that we have longitudinal MyCog data, this allows us to look at change in MyCog score, which should be a purer measure of subjective cognitive decline, assuming that the contribution of anxiety traits remains stable. In our statistical models (see below), we have adjusted for trait anxiety score, which has a possible range between 20 and 80 (with higher scores indicating greater anxiety). The score is derived from 20 questions asking participants about how they feel generally (e.g. ‘I lack self-confidence’).

Life-course and clinical variables

The nature of the NSHD has facilitated prospective collection of extensive life-course data. We included the following variables because they have previously been shown to be correlated with cognitive function in later life.32,51

Childhood cognitive ability was measured with tests of verbal and non-verbal ability at ages 8, 11 and 15 years, standardized into z-scores as previously described.32 We have used the z-score from age 8 years (or ages 11 or 15 years if earlier data were missing).

Education was classified into five categories based on highest qualification at age 26 years: no qualification, below O-levels (vocational), O-levels and equivalents (ordinary), A-levels and equivalents (advanced) or higher (degree and equivalents).

Socioeconomic position was derived from the occupations of participants (based on data collected at age 53 years, or earlier where this was missing), classified into six categories: unskilled, partly skilled, skilled manual, skilled non-manual, intermediate or professional.32

Participants were classified as having a neurological or major psychiatric condition (including dementia and mild cognitive impairment) using previously described criteria32 (for specific diagnoses, see Supplementary Fig. 1). We refer to participants not meeting these criteria as cognitively normal. This does not imply that all participants with a neurological or major psychiatric condition necessarily had a measurable cognitive impairment.

Statistical analysis

Analyses were conducted using Stata 18 (StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA). Statistical significance was set at P < 0.05. Model assumptions were checked by examination of residuals.

Amyloid pathology and brain and hippocampal atrophy

We tested the following hypotheses in cognitively normal participants with complete biomarker data (n = 306; Supplementary Fig. 1): (i) poorer performance on the ALF test (i.e. greater forgetting) would be associated with amyloid pathology and brain and hippocampal atrophy; and (ii) ALF-Drawingoutline would be more sensitive to these pathologies than ALF-Drawingtotal and ALF-Drawingdetail.

Multivariable regression models were used to investigate associations between ALF and amyloid pathology and cerebral atrophy rates (whole-brain and hippocampal) and to explore interactions between these predictors. Eight models were fitted for each of the four ALF scores (ALF-Drawingtotal, ALF-Drawingoutline, ALF-Drawingdetail and ALF-FNAME). Models 1–4 use amyloid status (positive/negative) as a dichotomous measure of amyloid pathology. Models 5–8 essentially repeat Models 1–4 but with SUVR as the continuous measure of amyloid pathology: Model 1, amyloid status and whole-brain atrophy rate; Model 2, amyloid status and hippocampal atrophy rate; Model 3, interaction between amyloid status and whole-brain atrophy rate; Model 4, interaction between amyloid status and hippocampal atrophy rate; Model 5, SUVR and whole-brain atrophy rate; Model 6, SUVR and hippocampal atrophy rate; Model 7, interaction between SUVR and whole-brain atrophy rate; and Model 8, interaction between SUVR and hippocampal atrophy rate.

Given that the distributions of the outcome variables were somewhat skewed, bootstrapping was used to produce bias-corrected and accelerated 95% confidence intervals (CIs) from 2000 replications. All models adjusted for the following potential confounders: sex, age at ALF assessment, childhood cognitive ability, education, socioeconomic position, APOE-ε4 genotype (carrier or non-carrier), white matter hyperintensity volume and total intracranial volume (an index of head size).

Although our main outcomes were the ALF scores, we also examined performance on each trial of the tasks, including the recognition tests (described above and summarized in Fig. 1). Our purposes were to build on our findings of associations between these pathologies and ALF, by addressing the following questions. (i) Did the difference between the amyloid groups on complex figure drawing emerge only after 7 days (i.e. no evidence of differences after a short delay of 30 min, consistent with the definition of ALF)? (ii) Were amyloid-related deficits after 7 days specific to recall or were deficits also seen in recognition memory? For each measure, a multivariable regression model was fitted where the outcome was the score (converted into percentage correct) and the predictors were amyloid status and whole-brain atrophy rate (analogous to Model 1 above). For variables where the distributions of the scores were skewed, bootstrapping was used to produce bias-corrected and accelerated 95% CIs from 2000 replications. All models adjusted for the same potential confounders as listed above.

Subjective cognitive decline

In all cognitively normal participants (n = 377; Supplementary Fig. 1), we tested the hypothesis that poorer performance on the ALF test would be associated with greater subjective cognitive decline.

Multivariable regression models were fitted, where the outcomes were the four ALF scores (ALF-Drawingtotal, ALF-Drawingoutline, ALF-Drawingdetail and ALF-FNAME) and the predictors were: (i) MyCog score (cross-sectional, administered at follow-up on the same day as the ALF assessment); and (ii) ‘change in MyCog score’ (i.e. follow-up minus baseline score). Given that the distributions of the outcome variables were somewhat skewed, bootstrapping was used to produce bias-corrected and accelerated 95% CIs from 2000 replications. All models adjusted for the following potential confounders: sex, age at ALF assessment, childhood cognitive ability, education and socioeconomic position. For models that used change in MyCog score, we additionally adjusted for the duration of the interval between baseline and follow-up assessments.

Based on our previous finding of an association between MyCog score and trait anxiety at baseline (described earlier),49 we reran the above models adjusting for follow-up trait anxiety score.

Results

Participants

Of the 429 participants with ALF scores available, 377 were classified as cognitively normal, of whom 306 had complete biomarker data (Supplementary Fig. 1). Ninety-five (31%) of these were amyloid-positive. Participant characteristics and descriptive statistics for ALF scores are provided in Table 1 for the n = 306 sample, subdivided by amyloid status (see Supplementary Table 1 for a version of this table that includes all 429 participants). Note that 304 of the 306 participants were assessed in person, with the remaining two (both amyloid-negative) assessed by video call. No significant differences were identified between amyloid-positive and amyloid-negative groups in terms of age at assessment, childhood cognitive ability or socioeconomic position. Amyloid-positive participants were slightly less well educated and were more likely to be APOE-ε4 carriers.

Table 1

Participant characteristics and accelerated long-term forgetting scores in cognitively-normal participants with complete biomarker data (n = 306)

Characteristicβ-Amyloid-negativeβ-Amyloid-positive
n21195
Percentage female49.852.6
Age at ALF assessment, years: mean, SD, (range)72.9, 0.64, (71.9, 74.7)72.9, 0.59, (71.9, 74.0)
Childhood cognitive ability, z-score: mean, SD, (range)0.48, 0.71, (−1.59, 2.47)0.34, 0.71, (−1.37, 2.50)
Highest educational qualification, %a
 None12.820.0
 Below O-levels (vocational)4.73.2
 O-levels or equivalent26.130.5
 A-levels or equivalent34.632.6
 Degree or equivalent21.813.7
Socioeconomic position, %
 Unskilled0.50
 Partly skilled5.25.3
 Skilled manual9.56.3
 Skilled non-manual21.824.2
 Intermediate52.655.8
 Professional10.48.4
Percentage APOE-ε4 carriersa19.154.7
Neuroimaging follow-up interval, years: mean, SD, (range)2.4, 0.2, (2.0, 3.2)2.4, 0.2, (2.2, 3.1)
White matter hyperintensity volume, ml: median, IQR, (range)4.1, 2.0–9.1, (0.1, 44.0)4.1, 2.0–9.1, (0.1, 44.0)
Whole-brain atrophy rateb, ml/year: mean, SD, (range)−5.7, 3.1, (−15.9, 3.6)−6.3, 2.9, (−14.1, −0.7)
Hippocampal atrophy rateb, ml/year: mean, SD, (range)−0.04, 0.04, (−0.22, 0.07)−0.04, 0.04, (−0.17, 0.04)
MyCog subjective cognitive decline score (out of 24): mean, SD, (range)4.5, 3.9, (0, 18)5.2, 4.0, (0, 21)
Change in MyCog score: mean, SD, (range)0.36, 3.1, (−10, 11)0.33, 2.7, (−7, 10)
Preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite: mean, SD, (range)0.09, 0.62, (−1.8, 1.5)0.02, 0.66, (−1.9, 1.5)
 Accelerated long-term forgetting scores (%): mean, SD, [median], (range)
 Complex figure drawingtotal79, 16, [80], (12–122)77, 19, [76], (0–111)
 Complex figure drawingoutlinea96, 16, [100], (14–200)89, 17, [95], (0–118)
 Complex figure drawingdetail72, 22, [71], (11–157)71, 23, [71], (0–125)
 Face–name77, 21, [79], (0–200)76, 22, [79], (0–129)
Characteristicβ-Amyloid-negativeβ-Amyloid-positive
n21195
Percentage female49.852.6
Age at ALF assessment, years: mean, SD, (range)72.9, 0.64, (71.9, 74.7)72.9, 0.59, (71.9, 74.0)
Childhood cognitive ability, z-score: mean, SD, (range)0.48, 0.71, (−1.59, 2.47)0.34, 0.71, (−1.37, 2.50)
Highest educational qualification, %a
 None12.820.0
 Below O-levels (vocational)4.73.2
 O-levels or equivalent26.130.5
 A-levels or equivalent34.632.6
 Degree or equivalent21.813.7
Socioeconomic position, %
 Unskilled0.50
 Partly skilled5.25.3
 Skilled manual9.56.3
 Skilled non-manual21.824.2
 Intermediate52.655.8
 Professional10.48.4
Percentage APOE-ε4 carriersa19.154.7
Neuroimaging follow-up interval, years: mean, SD, (range)2.4, 0.2, (2.0, 3.2)2.4, 0.2, (2.2, 3.1)
White matter hyperintensity volume, ml: median, IQR, (range)4.1, 2.0–9.1, (0.1, 44.0)4.1, 2.0–9.1, (0.1, 44.0)
Whole-brain atrophy rateb, ml/year: mean, SD, (range)−5.7, 3.1, (−15.9, 3.6)−6.3, 2.9, (−14.1, −0.7)
Hippocampal atrophy rateb, ml/year: mean, SD, (range)−0.04, 0.04, (−0.22, 0.07)−0.04, 0.04, (−0.17, 0.04)
MyCog subjective cognitive decline score (out of 24): mean, SD, (range)4.5, 3.9, (0, 18)5.2, 4.0, (0, 21)
Change in MyCog score: mean, SD, (range)0.36, 3.1, (−10, 11)0.33, 2.7, (−7, 10)
Preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite: mean, SD, (range)0.09, 0.62, (−1.8, 1.5)0.02, 0.66, (−1.9, 1.5)
 Accelerated long-term forgetting scores (%): mean, SD, [median], (range)
 Complex figure drawingtotal79, 16, [80], (12–122)77, 19, [76], (0–111)
 Complex figure drawingoutlinea96, 16, [100], (14–200)89, 17, [95], (0–118)
 Complex figure drawingdetail72, 22, [71], (11–157)71, 23, [71], (0–125)
 Face–name77, 21, [79], (0–200)76, 22, [79], (0–129)

ALF = accelerated long-term forgetting; IQR = interquartile range; SD = standard deviation.

aBased on t-test, ranksum tests or χ2 tests, as appropriate, the difference between amyloid-positive and amyloid-negative groups was statistically significant for education, APOE-ε4 and complex figure drawingoutline.

bAtrophy is represented as change in volume, with negative numbers indicating volume loss.

Table 1

Participant characteristics and accelerated long-term forgetting scores in cognitively-normal participants with complete biomarker data (n = 306)

Characteristicβ-Amyloid-negativeβ-Amyloid-positive
n21195
Percentage female49.852.6
Age at ALF assessment, years: mean, SD, (range)72.9, 0.64, (71.9, 74.7)72.9, 0.59, (71.9, 74.0)
Childhood cognitive ability, z-score: mean, SD, (range)0.48, 0.71, (−1.59, 2.47)0.34, 0.71, (−1.37, 2.50)
Highest educational qualification, %a
 None12.820.0
 Below O-levels (vocational)4.73.2
 O-levels or equivalent26.130.5
 A-levels or equivalent34.632.6
 Degree or equivalent21.813.7
Socioeconomic position, %
 Unskilled0.50
 Partly skilled5.25.3
 Skilled manual9.56.3
 Skilled non-manual21.824.2
 Intermediate52.655.8
 Professional10.48.4
Percentage APOE-ε4 carriersa19.154.7
Neuroimaging follow-up interval, years: mean, SD, (range)2.4, 0.2, (2.0, 3.2)2.4, 0.2, (2.2, 3.1)
White matter hyperintensity volume, ml: median, IQR, (range)4.1, 2.0–9.1, (0.1, 44.0)4.1, 2.0–9.1, (0.1, 44.0)
Whole-brain atrophy rateb, ml/year: mean, SD, (range)−5.7, 3.1, (−15.9, 3.6)−6.3, 2.9, (−14.1, −0.7)
Hippocampal atrophy rateb, ml/year: mean, SD, (range)−0.04, 0.04, (−0.22, 0.07)−0.04, 0.04, (−0.17, 0.04)
MyCog subjective cognitive decline score (out of 24): mean, SD, (range)4.5, 3.9, (0, 18)5.2, 4.0, (0, 21)
Change in MyCog score: mean, SD, (range)0.36, 3.1, (−10, 11)0.33, 2.7, (−7, 10)
Preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite: mean, SD, (range)0.09, 0.62, (−1.8, 1.5)0.02, 0.66, (−1.9, 1.5)
 Accelerated long-term forgetting scores (%): mean, SD, [median], (range)
 Complex figure drawingtotal79, 16, [80], (12–122)77, 19, [76], (0–111)
 Complex figure drawingoutlinea96, 16, [100], (14–200)89, 17, [95], (0–118)
 Complex figure drawingdetail72, 22, [71], (11–157)71, 23, [71], (0–125)
 Face–name77, 21, [79], (0–200)76, 22, [79], (0–129)
Characteristicβ-Amyloid-negativeβ-Amyloid-positive
n21195
Percentage female49.852.6
Age at ALF assessment, years: mean, SD, (range)72.9, 0.64, (71.9, 74.7)72.9, 0.59, (71.9, 74.0)
Childhood cognitive ability, z-score: mean, SD, (range)0.48, 0.71, (−1.59, 2.47)0.34, 0.71, (−1.37, 2.50)
Highest educational qualification, %a
 None12.820.0
 Below O-levels (vocational)4.73.2
 O-levels or equivalent26.130.5
 A-levels or equivalent34.632.6
 Degree or equivalent21.813.7
Socioeconomic position, %
 Unskilled0.50
 Partly skilled5.25.3
 Skilled manual9.56.3
 Skilled non-manual21.824.2
 Intermediate52.655.8
 Professional10.48.4
Percentage APOE-ε4 carriersa19.154.7
Neuroimaging follow-up interval, years: mean, SD, (range)2.4, 0.2, (2.0, 3.2)2.4, 0.2, (2.2, 3.1)
White matter hyperintensity volume, ml: median, IQR, (range)4.1, 2.0–9.1, (0.1, 44.0)4.1, 2.0–9.1, (0.1, 44.0)
Whole-brain atrophy rateb, ml/year: mean, SD, (range)−5.7, 3.1, (−15.9, 3.6)−6.3, 2.9, (−14.1, −0.7)
Hippocampal atrophy rateb, ml/year: mean, SD, (range)−0.04, 0.04, (−0.22, 0.07)−0.04, 0.04, (−0.17, 0.04)
MyCog subjective cognitive decline score (out of 24): mean, SD, (range)4.5, 3.9, (0, 18)5.2, 4.0, (0, 21)
Change in MyCog score: mean, SD, (range)0.36, 3.1, (−10, 11)0.33, 2.7, (−7, 10)
Preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite: mean, SD, (range)0.09, 0.62, (−1.8, 1.5)0.02, 0.66, (−1.9, 1.5)
 Accelerated long-term forgetting scores (%): mean, SD, [median], (range)
 Complex figure drawingtotal79, 16, [80], (12–122)77, 19, [76], (0–111)
 Complex figure drawingoutlinea96, 16, [100], (14–200)89, 17, [95], (0–118)
 Complex figure drawingdetail72, 22, [71], (11–157)71, 23, [71], (0–125)
 Face–name77, 21, [79], (0–200)76, 22, [79], (0–129)

ALF = accelerated long-term forgetting; IQR = interquartile range; SD = standard deviation.

aBased on t-test, ranksum tests or χ2 tests, as appropriate, the difference between amyloid-positive and amyloid-negative groups was statistically significant for education, APOE-ε4 and complex figure drawingoutline.

bAtrophy is represented as change in volume, with negative numbers indicating volume loss.

Amyloid pathology and brain and hippocampal atrophy

Amyloid status was associated with ALF-Drawingoutline (Table 2): amyloid-positive participants showed greater forgetting of the outline of the complex figure [90% of the outline was retained by amyloid-positives (95% CIs 85, 94) and 95% (92, 99) by amyloid-negatives (marginal means from the regression model, P < 0.01)]. Consistent with our hypothesis, ALF-Drawingoutline was the only complex figure score to show this effect; ALF-Drawingtotal and ALF-Drawingdetail did not exhibit a statistically significant difference between amyloid-positive and amyloid-negative groups (Table 2). ALF-FNAME also did not differ between amyloid-positive and amyloid-negative groups (Table 2).

Table 2

Predictors of accelerated long-term forgetting scores from the complex figure drawing and face–name tests in cognitively normal participants (n = 306)

ModelPredictorCoefficient (95% confidence intervals)
(% retained after 7 days, relative to after 30 min)
ALF-DrawingtotalALF-DrawingoutlineALF-DrawingdetailALF-FNAME
Model 1aAmyloid status (negative as reference)−1.1 (−5.1, 3.4)−5.6 (−9.5, −1.7)c1.0 (−4.7, 6.7)1.5 (−4.8, 7.1)
Model 1Brain atrophy rate, ml/year0.6 (−0.2, 1.4)0.1 (−0.7, 0.8)0.8 (−0.1, 1.8)0.5 (−0.3, 1.4)
Model 2Hippocampal atrophy rate, ml/year42.5 (−5.3, 95.2)10.3 (−41.0, 71.1)61.0 (−1.3, 126.2)76.5 (12.0, 141.5)b
Model 3Interaction between amyloid status and whole-brain atrophy rate1.9 (0.3, 3.7)b2.2 (1.1, 4.4)c1.8 (−0.2, 3.9)1.6 (−0.1, 3.7)
 Whole-brain atrophy rate in amyloid-positive1.9 (0.5, 3.7)b1.6 (0.6, 3.8)b2.2 (0.4, 4.0)b1.7 (−0.2, 3.5)
 Whole-brain atrophy rate in amyloid-negative0.1 (−0.9, 0.8)−0.6 (−1.5, 0.1)0.3 (−0.8, 1.4)0.1 (−1.0, 1.0)
Model 4Interaction between amyloid status and hippocampal atrophy rate47.6 (−57.6, 192.4)146.0 (49.4, 300.7)c−8.3 (−157.0, 163.8)32.5 (−110.1, 180.1)
 Hippocampal atrophy rate in amyloid-positive75.5 (−18.6, 208.9)111.4 (−27.4, 266.9)b55.3 (−70.8, 195.2)101.3 (−9.7, 257.5)
 Hippocampal atrophy rate in amyloid-negative27.9 (−28.3, 77.4)−34.6 (−93.0, 14.6)63.5 (−8.6, 133.5)65.7 (−7.4, 141.7)
ModelPredictorCoefficient (95% confidence intervals)
(% retained after 7 days, relative to after 30 min)
ALF-DrawingtotalALF-DrawingoutlineALF-DrawingdetailALF-FNAME
Model 1aAmyloid status (negative as reference)−1.1 (−5.1, 3.4)−5.6 (−9.5, −1.7)c1.0 (−4.7, 6.7)1.5 (−4.8, 7.1)
Model 1Brain atrophy rate, ml/year0.6 (−0.2, 1.4)0.1 (−0.7, 0.8)0.8 (−0.1, 1.8)0.5 (−0.3, 1.4)
Model 2Hippocampal atrophy rate, ml/year42.5 (−5.3, 95.2)10.3 (−41.0, 71.1)61.0 (−1.3, 126.2)76.5 (12.0, 141.5)b
Model 3Interaction between amyloid status and whole-brain atrophy rate1.9 (0.3, 3.7)b2.2 (1.1, 4.4)c1.8 (−0.2, 3.9)1.6 (−0.1, 3.7)
 Whole-brain atrophy rate in amyloid-positive1.9 (0.5, 3.7)b1.6 (0.6, 3.8)b2.2 (0.4, 4.0)b1.7 (−0.2, 3.5)
 Whole-brain atrophy rate in amyloid-negative0.1 (−0.9, 0.8)−0.6 (−1.5, 0.1)0.3 (−0.8, 1.4)0.1 (−1.0, 1.0)
Model 4Interaction between amyloid status and hippocampal atrophy rate47.6 (−57.6, 192.4)146.0 (49.4, 300.7)c−8.3 (−157.0, 163.8)32.5 (−110.1, 180.1)
 Hippocampal atrophy rate in amyloid-positive75.5 (−18.6, 208.9)111.4 (−27.4, 266.9)b55.3 (−70.8, 195.2)101.3 (−9.7, 257.5)
 Hippocampal atrophy rate in amyloid-negative27.9 (−28.3, 77.4)−34.6 (−93.0, 14.6)63.5 (−8.6, 133.5)65.7 (−7.4, 141.7)

P < 0.05 or P > 0.05 and P < 0.01 or P > 0.01 was inferred from bootstrapped 95% and 99% confidence intervals. Atrophy is represented as change in volume (with negative values representing volume loss), hence positive regression coefficients indicate associations between volume loss and poorer accelerated long-term forgetting (ALF) scores. See the ‘Materials and methods’ section for details of Models 1–4. Multivariable regression models were used, hence each association is independent of all others. All models also included adjustment for sex, age at assessment, education, childhood cognitive ability, socioeconomic position, APOE-ε4 genotype, white matter hyperintensity volume and head size (total intracranial volume). For details of the four ALF scores, see the ‘Materials and methods’ section. Values in bold are significant at P < 0.05 or P < 0.01. FNAME = face–name associative memory exam.

aThe coefficients for amyloid status are essentially the same in Model 1 (shown here) and Model 2 (not shown).

bSignificant at P < 0.05.

cSignificant at P < 0.01.

Table 2

Predictors of accelerated long-term forgetting scores from the complex figure drawing and face–name tests in cognitively normal participants (n = 306)

ModelPredictorCoefficient (95% confidence intervals)
(% retained after 7 days, relative to after 30 min)
ALF-DrawingtotalALF-DrawingoutlineALF-DrawingdetailALF-FNAME
Model 1aAmyloid status (negative as reference)−1.1 (−5.1, 3.4)−5.6 (−9.5, −1.7)c1.0 (−4.7, 6.7)1.5 (−4.8, 7.1)
Model 1Brain atrophy rate, ml/year0.6 (−0.2, 1.4)0.1 (−0.7, 0.8)0.8 (−0.1, 1.8)0.5 (−0.3, 1.4)
Model 2Hippocampal atrophy rate, ml/year42.5 (−5.3, 95.2)10.3 (−41.0, 71.1)61.0 (−1.3, 126.2)76.5 (12.0, 141.5)b
Model 3Interaction between amyloid status and whole-brain atrophy rate1.9 (0.3, 3.7)b2.2 (1.1, 4.4)c1.8 (−0.2, 3.9)1.6 (−0.1, 3.7)
 Whole-brain atrophy rate in amyloid-positive1.9 (0.5, 3.7)b1.6 (0.6, 3.8)b2.2 (0.4, 4.0)b1.7 (−0.2, 3.5)
 Whole-brain atrophy rate in amyloid-negative0.1 (−0.9, 0.8)−0.6 (−1.5, 0.1)0.3 (−0.8, 1.4)0.1 (−1.0, 1.0)
Model 4Interaction between amyloid status and hippocampal atrophy rate47.6 (−57.6, 192.4)146.0 (49.4, 300.7)c−8.3 (−157.0, 163.8)32.5 (−110.1, 180.1)
 Hippocampal atrophy rate in amyloid-positive75.5 (−18.6, 208.9)111.4 (−27.4, 266.9)b55.3 (−70.8, 195.2)101.3 (−9.7, 257.5)
 Hippocampal atrophy rate in amyloid-negative27.9 (−28.3, 77.4)−34.6 (−93.0, 14.6)63.5 (−8.6, 133.5)65.7 (−7.4, 141.7)
ModelPredictorCoefficient (95% confidence intervals)
(% retained after 7 days, relative to after 30 min)
ALF-DrawingtotalALF-DrawingoutlineALF-DrawingdetailALF-FNAME
Model 1aAmyloid status (negative as reference)−1.1 (−5.1, 3.4)−5.6 (−9.5, −1.7)c1.0 (−4.7, 6.7)1.5 (−4.8, 7.1)
Model 1Brain atrophy rate, ml/year0.6 (−0.2, 1.4)0.1 (−0.7, 0.8)0.8 (−0.1, 1.8)0.5 (−0.3, 1.4)
Model 2Hippocampal atrophy rate, ml/year42.5 (−5.3, 95.2)10.3 (−41.0, 71.1)61.0 (−1.3, 126.2)76.5 (12.0, 141.5)b
Model 3Interaction between amyloid status and whole-brain atrophy rate1.9 (0.3, 3.7)b2.2 (1.1, 4.4)c1.8 (−0.2, 3.9)1.6 (−0.1, 3.7)
 Whole-brain atrophy rate in amyloid-positive1.9 (0.5, 3.7)b1.6 (0.6, 3.8)b2.2 (0.4, 4.0)b1.7 (−0.2, 3.5)
 Whole-brain atrophy rate in amyloid-negative0.1 (−0.9, 0.8)−0.6 (−1.5, 0.1)0.3 (−0.8, 1.4)0.1 (−1.0, 1.0)
Model 4Interaction between amyloid status and hippocampal atrophy rate47.6 (−57.6, 192.4)146.0 (49.4, 300.7)c−8.3 (−157.0, 163.8)32.5 (−110.1, 180.1)
 Hippocampal atrophy rate in amyloid-positive75.5 (−18.6, 208.9)111.4 (−27.4, 266.9)b55.3 (−70.8, 195.2)101.3 (−9.7, 257.5)
 Hippocampal atrophy rate in amyloid-negative27.9 (−28.3, 77.4)−34.6 (−93.0, 14.6)63.5 (−8.6, 133.5)65.7 (−7.4, 141.7)

P < 0.05 or P > 0.05 and P < 0.01 or P > 0.01 was inferred from bootstrapped 95% and 99% confidence intervals. Atrophy is represented as change in volume (with negative values representing volume loss), hence positive regression coefficients indicate associations between volume loss and poorer accelerated long-term forgetting (ALF) scores. See the ‘Materials and methods’ section for details of Models 1–4. Multivariable regression models were used, hence each association is independent of all others. All models also included adjustment for sex, age at assessment, education, childhood cognitive ability, socioeconomic position, APOE-ε4 genotype, white matter hyperintensity volume and head size (total intracranial volume). For details of the four ALF scores, see the ‘Materials and methods’ section. Values in bold are significant at P < 0.05 or P < 0.01. FNAME = face–name associative memory exam.

aThe coefficients for amyloid status are essentially the same in Model 1 (shown here) and Model 2 (not shown).

bSignificant at P < 0.05.

cSignificant at P < 0.01.

Figure 4 illustrates the trial-by-trial recall performance of amyloid-positive and amyloid-negative groups (see also Supplementary Table 2), showing that the only trial with a statistically significant difference between the groups was the 7-day recall of the complex figure drawing ‘outline’, for which amyloid-positive participants scored lower. This poorer 7-day recall, despite unimpaired performance on 30-min recall, is what gives rise to their lower ALF-Drawingoutline score.

Means and 95% confidence intervals for recall on complex figure drawing (A) and face–name test (FNAME-12) (B). The means and confidence intervals are predictions from multivariable regression models (see the ‘Materials and methods’ section) adjusted for sex, age at assessment, childhood cognitive ability, education, socioeconomic position, APOE-ε4 genotype, brain atrophy rate (in millilitres per year), white matter hyperintensity volume and total intracranial volume. Bootstrapping was used to produce bias-corrected and accelerated confidence intervals from 2000 replications. Asterisk indicates statistically significant difference between amyloid-positive and -negative groups (P < 0.05) for 7-day recall of the complex figure outline items. Aβ = amyloid-β.
Figure 4

Means and 95% confidence intervals for recall on complex figure drawing (A) and face–name test (FNAME-12) (B). The means and confidence intervals are predictions from multivariable regression models (see the ‘Materials and methods’ section) adjusted for sex, age at assessment, childhood cognitive ability, education, socioeconomic position, APOE-ε4 genotype, brain atrophy rate (in millilitres per year), white matter hyperintensity volume and total intracranial volume. Bootstrapping was used to produce bias-corrected and accelerated confidence intervals from 2000 replications. Asterisk indicates statistically significant difference between amyloid-positive and -negative groups (P < 0.05) for 7-day recall of the complex figure outline items. Aβ = amyloid-β.

In analyses of trial-by-trial recognition performance (see also Supplementary Table 2), we found no statistically significant differences between amyloid-positive and amyloid-negative groups in terms of 7-day recognition memory [complex figure recognition: amyloid-positive, mean = 86.2% (95% CIs 80.9, 91.3); amyloid-negative, 87.1% (84.4, 89.6), P > 0.05; FNAME recognition of names and occupations: amyloid-positive, 88.7% (85.9, 91.2); amyloid-negative, 88.8% (87.4, 90.2), P > 0.05].

Whole-brain and hippocampal atrophy did not have statistically significant associations with ALF scores on the complex figure drawing task (adjusting for amyloid status), although results were in the expected direction of accelerated forgetting with faster atrophy rates (Table 2). However, there were interactions between amyloid status and atrophy rates, such that the associations between faster atrophy and accelerated forgetting were seen in those who were amyloid-positive but not those who were amyloid-negative (Table 2). In particular, statistically significant interactions were observed for ALF-Drawingoutline (whole-brain and hippocampal atrophy rates) and ALF-Drawingtotal (whole-brain atrophy rate) (Table 2 and Fig. 5). In other words, on the complex figure drawing task we found evidence of a correlation between degree of ALF and rates of neurodegeneration, in the context of elevated amyloid pathology.

Associations of whole-brain and hippocampal atrophy with accelerated long-term forgetting of the complex figure drawing. (A) Whole-brain atrophy rate with Drawingtotal. (B) Whole-brain atrophy rate with Drawingoutline. (C) Hippocampal atrophy rate with Drawingoutline. Note that a score of >100% is possible if participants recalled more material after 7 days than after 30 min. The solid line represents the line of best fit from the multivariate regression model, adjusted for sex, age at assessment, childhood cognitive ability, education, socioeconomic position, APOE genotype (ε4 carrier/non-carrier), amyloid status, white matter hyperintensity volume and total intracranial volume. The shaded area represents its 95% confidence intervals. Markers show the unadjusted raw data. Negative values for whole-brain and hippocampal volume change represent volume loss (atrophy). Aβ = amyloid-β.
Figure 5

Associations of whole-brain and hippocampal atrophy with accelerated long-term forgetting of the complex figure drawing. (A) Whole-brain atrophy rate with Drawingtotal. (B) Whole-brain atrophy rate with Drawingoutline. (C) Hippocampal atrophy rate with Drawingoutline. Note that a score of >100% is possible if participants recalled more material after 7 days than after 30 min. The solid line represents the line of best fit from the multivariate regression model, adjusted for sex, age at assessment, childhood cognitive ability, education, socioeconomic position, APOE genotype (ε4 carrier/non-carrier), amyloid status, white matter hyperintensity volume and total intracranial volume. The shaded area represents its 95% confidence intervals. Markers show the unadjusted raw data. Negative values for whole-brain and hippocampal volume change represent volume loss (atrophy). Aβ = amyloid-β.

On the face–name test, there was an association between faster hippocampal atrophy and accelerated forgetting (Table 2 and Supplementary Fig. 2), which did not differ according to amyloid status (i.e. no statistically significant interaction; Table 2). Brain atrophy showed a non-significant trend in the same direction (i.e. lower ALF-FNAME scores with faster atrophy), with a stronger association in the amyloid-positive group, although this interaction was not statistically significant (Table 2).

When rerunning the models with the continuous measure of amyloid pathology (SUVR), results were similar (Supplementary Table 3).

Of the demographic and life-course factors included in the models, only education was a statistically significant predictor of performance, with higher educational attainment associated with better ALF scores on complex figure drawing (coefficients not reported). APOE-ε4 and white matter hyperintensity volume had no independent effects on any outcome (coefficients not reported).

Subjective cognitive decline

Higher cross-sectional MyCog score (i.e. greater subjective cognitive concerns) was associated with greater forgetting on ALF-Drawingtotal [coefficient = −0.45 per MyCog point (95% CIs −0.85, −0.05), P < 0.05; Fig. 6A] and ALF-Drawingdetail [−0.58 (−1.16, −0.02), P < 0.05]. Results for ALF-Drawingoutline were similar, although non-statistically significant [−0.34 (−0.73, 0.07)]. There was no evidence of an association between MyCog score and ALF on the face–name test [ALF-FNAME = −0.19 (−0.79, 0.35)].

Associations in cognitively normal participants between subjective cognitive decline and accelerated long-term forgetting of the complex figure drawing (Drawingtotal). (A) Cross-sectional subjective cognitive concerns. (B) Change in subjective cognitive concerns since baseline (∼2.4 years earlier). Note that a score of >100% is possible if participants recalled more material after 7 days than after 30 min. In each graph, the solid line represents the line of best fit from the multivariate regression model, adjusted for sex, age at assessment, childhood cognitive ability, education, socioeconomic position, APOE genotype (ε4 carrier/non-carrier), amyloid status, white matter hyperintensity volume and total intracranial volume. The shaded area represents its 95% confidence intervals. Markers show the unadjusted raw data.
Figure 6

Associations in cognitively normal participants between subjective cognitive decline and accelerated long-term forgetting of the complex figure drawing (Drawingtotal). (A) Cross-sectional subjective cognitive concerns. (B) Change in subjective cognitive concerns since baseline (∼2.4 years earlier). Note that a score of >100% is possible if participants recalled more material after 7 days than after 30 min. In each graph, the solid line represents the line of best fit from the multivariate regression model, adjusted for sex, age at assessment, childhood cognitive ability, education, socioeconomic position, APOE genotype (ε4 carrier/non-carrier), amyloid status, white matter hyperintensity volume and total intracranial volume. The shaded area represents its 95% confidence intervals. Markers show the unadjusted raw data.

Greater increase in MyCog score (i.e. increase in subjective cognitive concerns over the last ∼2.4 years) was associated with greater forgetting on ALF-Drawingtotal [−0.56 per increase of one MyCog point (95% CIs −1.18, −0.03), P < 0.05; Fig. 6B] and ALF-Drawingoutline [−0.52 (−1.20, −0.03), P < 0.05], but not ALF-Drawingdetail [−0.51 (−1.38, 0.24)] and ALF-FNAME [0.06 (CIs −0.67, 0.78)].

Adjusting for trait anxiety made no material difference to these results (results not shown).

Discussion

This study examined ALF in cognitively normal ∼73-year-olds using visual and verbal memory tests. Our results indicate that complex figure drawing reveals ALF in individuals with amyloid pathology, accelerated rates of neurodegeneration and subjective cognitive decline. Specifically, amyloid-positive individuals showed normal recall after a standard 30 min testing delay but forgot a greater proportion of the outline structure of the complex figure over 7 days. Among individuals with elevated amyloid, faster rates of whole-brain and hippocampal atrophy were associated with greater degree of ALF on the complex figure. Accelerated forgetting of the complex figure was also associated with higher subjective cognitive decline, despite our study including only those classified as cognitively normal. This suggests that complex figure drawing with 7-day delayed recall could be a useful test for identifying individuals in the preclinical stage of Ad.

However, the second ALF task (the face–name test) did not follow the same pattern of results. Degree of forgetting on this test was not associated with either amyloid pathology or subjective cognitive decline. This discrepancy might reflect the differing psychometric properties, procedures and materials, and memory demands of the two tasks. For example, the tasks varied in the number of probes of the learned information (complex figure: three recall and one recognition probe; FNAME: five recall and two recognition probes), and although the number of long-delay periods was constant (one, at 7 days), multiple retrievals of learned information might reduce the rate of forgetting, because retrieval of probed features activates other associated features within that episode.2 We note that the standard face–name test (without 7-day recall) has previously been reported to be sensitive to amyloid pathology,52,53 although this was not the case in our cohort at age 70 years.32 However, we found evidence of an association between faster rates of hippocampal atrophy and greater forgetting over 7 days on this test. This is consistent with the pivotal role of the hippocampus in associative memory54 and with evidence of hippocampal activation during face–name associative memory tasks.55,56 To our knowledge, associations between hippocampal atrophy rates and performance on associative memory tests have not been reported before in cognitively normal older adults. The mean hippocampal atrophy rate in our sample (−0.04 ml/year, equivalent to ∼0.64% volume loss per year) was below the mean rate for elderly healthy controls of 1.4%/year reported in a meta-analysis,57 suggesting that even subtle excess hippocampal atrophy might have cognitive consequences. One probable reason why our analysis was able to detect this subtle association is that the age range of our participants was so narrow (71.9–74.8 years), minimizing the confounding effect of age.

Our results add to an emerging literature on ALF in the preclinical stage of Ad. Our analyses suggest that previous reports of associations between APOE-ε4 and ALF17,18 can be explained by amyloid pathology (which these studies did not measure), because APOE-ε4 had no independent effect in our study. In terms of how our results compare with evidence from the preclinical stage of autosomal dominant Ad,23-25 the evidence for ALF in that population appears to be stronger and more consistent (e.g. Weston et al.23 found that mutation carriers showed ALF on verbal and visual memory tests, on average 7 years before expected symptom onset). This difference might be because not all amyloid-positive older adults will develop Ad (whereas autosomal dominant mutations are almost fully penetrant), and the onset of symptoms might be many years away for our 73-year-old participants, based on the median age of onset of dementia of 84 years in the UK.58 Our finding of limited evidence for an association between subjective cognitive decline and ALF is consistent with the literature, where similar associations have been reported, but not consistently.19-21

A striking feature of performance on the complex figure drawing task was how much more easily the outline of the drawing was recalled than the internal details. This could potentially be explained by the phenomenon of memory selectivity,59 whereby human memory is adapted to work efficiently as a limited resource by remembering what is most important or most likely to enable the achievement of future goals. In our view, recalling the rough outline or skeleton of a diagram is analogous to recalling the gist of a conversation or the essence of a journal article without the precise words and details, with evidence that when recalling verbal stories, specific peripheral details are forgotten more quickly than the general gist.26 As such, it seems to demonstrate efficient strategic operation of memory, although complex figure performance is likely also to be influenced by perceptual organization factors, including global precedence (the identification of global over local features) and principles of grouping (the tendency to perceive patterns based on proximity, similarity and connectedness).60 In terms of why the outline might have been vulnerable to ALF in individuals with amyloid pathology, one factor of possible relevance is the organizational strategies that participants used when making their initial copy of the diagram. Anecdotally, we observed that participants tended to draw the outline first, which is consistent with the literature on organizational strategies in drawing.38 Deficits in organizational strategies of copying can mediate poorer delayed recall,61 hence it is possible that amyloid-positive participants might have been less well organized in their copying strategies. However, if that were the case, it would be surprising that we saw no hint of an amyloid-related recall deficit after a 30 min delay. Likewise, it is difficult to see how perceptual organization factors could influence 7-day recall but not 30-min recall. Instead, the divergence of performance over 7 days points us more towards differences in long-term memory storage and/or retrieval, discussed further below.

In terms of theoretical framework, little consensus has yet been reached within the field regarding the mechanisms underpinning ALF (for a review, see Rodini et al.10). In line with a qualitative distinction between early and late forgetting, and under the standard model, ALF has been taken to reflect a disruption of the slow stage of memory consolidation (replacement of hippocampal–neocortical connections with cortical–cortical connections).62 In line with a quantitative distinction between early and later forgetting, and under multiple trace theory, ALF in individuals who show normal or near normal learning and retention over short intervals has been taken to reflect subtle damage to a unitary consolidation mechanism.63

In line with this theoretical uncertainty, there has also been little agreement about the tests that should be used to detect ALF.10 However, the tests used in the present study (for the pragmatic reasons outlined in the ‘Materials and methods’ section) fulfil the majority of criteria and quality markers set out in the review by Elliot et al.,3 namely matching of patients and controls (in our case amyloid-positive and -negative individuals), use of visual and verbal material, inclusion of recall and recognition metrics, equated learning, avoidance of rehearsal (no warning given to participants of content of 7-day follow-up call) and avoidance of short-term memory contribution (by virtue of delay intervals).

One reason why Elliot et al.3 recommend that ALF studies should measure both recall and recognition is to allow researchers to distinguish between deficits of memory storage (impaired recall and impaired recognition) and deficits of retrieval (impaired recall but unimpaired recognition). Based on this, our results from the complex figure task could be argued to reflect a deficit of memory retrieval, because we found no difference between amyloid-positive and amyloid-negative groups on the 7-day recognition test. However, we think this interpretation might be too simplistic, because the subtle recall deficit observed in amyloid-positive participants applied only to the outline of the complex figure, not to recall of the figure as a whole. The stimuli in the recognition test are each based on approximately one quadrant of the figure (i.e. they each contain bits of the outline and internal details). Therefore, the recognition test removes some of the demand to remember the overall shape and cannot be used as a direct comparison to the novel ALF-Drawingoutline and ALF-Drawingdetail recall scores we created. Also, the recognition test had a very small range of possible scores (zero to four), which limits its sensitivity to differences between individuals.

Another clue to the processes underlying ALF in this cohort might come from the possible mechanisms by which amyloid pathology and brain atrophy could affect memory performance. Amyloid deposition leads to several changes within cell structure and function, including neuronal hyperexcitability, synaptic dysfunction and cell death.64-67 ALF in amyloid-positive participants could be mediated by any of these changes. Our results could also imply that the main impact of amyloid pathology on ALF is seen only after amyloid pathology leads to increased atrophy. This is suggested by our finding of associations between faster rates of atrophy and greater degree of ALF, which were driven primarily by amyloid-positive individuals. This implies that ALF is attributable to structural changes to regions involved in memory consolidation, storage and retrieval. It is also worth noting that, in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy, the presence of ALF has been considered to result from subclinical epileptiform activity and seizures.68 Although we have no evidence for seizure activity in our cohort, we are not able to rule out subclinical activity.

Our study had a number of limitations. First, our task procedure differed from some previous ALF studies in that we did not have a learning criterion (i.e. a minimum accuracy threshold for initial learning of the material, before the delayed recall trials).3 Some previous studies have set thresholds of 60%,69 75%,2,70 or 80%17,23 for word lists or stories. To our knowledge, no previous studies have applied a learning criterion to complex figure drawing tasks, because it is not feasible to score the drawings instantaneously (e.g.23,71). Our median accuracy for the immediate recall trials of complex figure drawing and face–name were 78% and 83%, respectively (Supplementary Table 2), indicating that most participants learnt the material to a good level. The lack of a learning criterion could have limited our ability to detect subtle ALF in participants whose immediate recall was relatively low, but we do not think this has influenced our main results, because immediate and 30-min recall were well matched between amyloid-positive and amyloid-negative groups.

Second, there are limitations relating to the representativeness of Insight 46 participants, as previously discussed,29,32 mainly that all participants are white British and tend to have slightly higher education and socioeconomic position than those not in the sub-study.29 However, the prevalence of amyloid pathology was in line with the literature for individuals of this age.72,73

Third, our study lacked a measure of tau pathology, limiting our ability to draw conclusions about relationships between preclinical Ad and ALF. Although cognitive deficits are observed in the context of amyloid pathology alone, these correlate more closely with the presence of tau.74,75 It will be of considerable interest to know whether the emergence of ALF in amyloid-positive individuals might be a very early sign of tau accumulation, with consequent neurodegeneration. We are currently following up many of these individuals with tau-PET imaging and will hopefully be able to address this important question in due course.

Conclusion

In summary, this study found associations in cognitively normal ∼73-year-olds between some aspects of performance on two ALF tests (complex figure drawing and face–name) and biomarkers of brain pathology (amyloid, brain atrophy rate and hippocampal atrophy rate) and subjective cognitive decline. On the complex figure drawing task, a distinction was seen between forgetting of the outline of the diagram versus forgetting of the detail, with the outline generally being much better learned and remembered (suggesting memory selectivity) but also being relatively more vulnerable to ALF in individuals with amyloid pathology. Overall, our findings suggest that subtle ALF might be a detectable early memory deficit in individuals who are on a preclinical Ad trajectory and might therefore predict risk of neurodegeneration and future cognitive decline.

Data availability

Data from the NSHD are curated and stored by the MRC Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL. Anonymized data will be shared by request from bona fide investigators (https://skylark.ucl.ac.uk/NSHD).

Acknowledgements

We thank participants both for their contributions to Insight 46 and for their commitments to research over the last seven decades. We are grateful to the radiographers and nuclear medicine physicians at the UCL Institute of Nuclear Medicine, and to the staff at the Leonard Wolfson Experimental Neurology Centre at UCL. We would like to acknowledge Dan Marcus and Rick Herrick for assistance with XNAT, and the Dementia Research Centre trials team for assistance with imaging quality control.

Funding

Insight 46 is funded by grants from Alzheimer’s Research UK (ARUK-PG2014-1946 and ARUK-PG2017-1946), Alzheimer’s Association (SG-666374-UK BIRTH COHORT), the Medical Research Council Dementias Platform UK (CSUB19166), The Wolfson Foundation (PR/ylr/18575), The Medical Research Council (MC_UU_10019/1 and MC_UU_10019/3) and Brain Research Trust (UCC14191). Florbetapir amyloid tracer was provided in kind by AVID Radiopharmaceuticals (a wholly owned subsidiary of Eli Lilly), who had no part in the design of the study. The funders of the study had no role in study design, data collection, analysis, interpretation, report writing or in the decision to submit the article for publication. A.K. was supported by a Wolfson Clinical Research Fellowship and acknowledges support from the Weston Brain Institute/Selfridges Foundation (UB170045) and the University College London Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre. N.C.F. acknowledges support from Alzheimer’s Research UK, the Alzheimer’s Society, Rosetrees Trust, UK Dementia Research Institute at University College London (UKDRI-1003), the National Institute for Health Research (Senior Investigator award) and University College London Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre. J.M.S. is supported by University College London Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre, Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council (EP/J020990/1), British Heart Foundation (PG/17/90/33415), EU’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (666992) and UK Dementia Research Institute at University College London (UKDRI-1003). D.M.C. is supported by the UK Dementia Research Institute, which receives its funding from DRI Ltd, funded by the UK Medical Research Council, Alzheimer’s Society and Alzheimer’s Research UK, the UKRI Innovation Scholars: Data Science Training in Health and Bioscience (MR/V03863X/1) and the National Institute for Health and Care Research University College London Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre.

Competing interests

N.C.F.’s research group has received payment for consultancy or for conducting studies from Biogen, Eli Lilly Research Laboratories, GE Healthcare and Roche. N.C.F. receives no personal compensation for the aforementioned activities. J.M.S. has received research funding from Avid Radiopharmaceuticals (a wholly owned subsidiary of Eli Lilly), has consulted for Roche Pharmaceuticals, Biogen, Merck and Eli Lilly, given educational lectures sponsored by GE Healthcare, Eli Lilly and Biogen, and serves on a Data Safety Monitoring Committee for Axon Neuroscience SE. The other authors report no competing interests.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Brain online.

References

1

Butler
 
C
,
Gilboa
 
A
,
Miller
 
L
.
Accelerated long-term forgetting
.
Cortex
.
2019
;
110
:
1
4
.

2

Baddeley
 
AD
,
Atkinson
 
AL
,
Hitch
 
GJ
,
Allen
 
RJ
.
Detecting accelerated long-term forgetting: A problem and some solutions
.
Cortex
.
2021
;
142
:
237
251
.

3

Elliott
 
G
,
Isaac
 
CL
,
Muhlert
 
N
.
Measuring forgetting: A critical review of accelerated long-term forgetting studies
.
Cortex
.
2014
;
54
:
16
32
.

4

Mameniškienė
 
R
,
Puteikis
 
K
,
Jasionis
 
A
,
Jatužis
 
D
.
A review of accelerated long-term forgetting in epilepsy
.
Brain Sci
.
2020
;
10
:
945
.

5

Stähli
 
NE
,
Bigi
 
S
,
Grunt
 
S
,
Lidzba
 
K
,
Studer
 
M
.
Systematic review of accelerated long-term forgetting in children and adolescents with neuropediatric diseases
.
Neurol Clin Pract
.
2022
;
12
:
10.1212/CPJ.0000000000200081
.

6

Baker
 
J
,
Savage
 
S
,
Milton
 
F
, et al.  
The syndrome of transient epileptic amnesia: A combined series of 115 cases and literature review
.
Brain Commun
.
2021
;
3
:
fcab038
.

7

Geurts
 
S
,
van der Werf
 
SP
,
Kwa
 
VIH
,
Kessels
 
RPC
.
Accelerated long-term forgetting after TIA or minor stroke: A more sensitive measure for detecting subtle memory dysfunction?
 
Cortex
.
2019
;
110
:
150
156
.

8

Lah
 
S
,
Black
 
C
,
Gascoigne
 
MB
,
Gott
 
C
,
Epps
 
A
,
Parry
 
L
.
Accelerated long-term forgetting is not epilepsy specific: Evidence from childhood traumatic brain injury
.
J Neurotrauma
.
2017
;
34
:
2536
2544
.

9

Stamate
 
A
,
Logie
 
RH
,
Baddeley
 
A
,
Della Sala
 
S
.
Forgetting in Alzheimer’s disease: Is it fast? Is it affected by repeated retrieval?
 
Neuropsychologia
 
2020
;
138
:
107351
.

10

Rodini
 
M
,
De Simone
 
MS
,
Caltagirone
 
C
,
Carlesimo
 
GA
.
Accelerated long-term forgetting in neurodegenerative disorders: A systematic review of the literature
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2022
;
141
:
104815
.

11

García-Martínez
 
M
,
Sánchez-Juan
 
P
,
Butler
 
CR
.
A review of accelerated long-term forgetting in Alzheimer’s disease: Current situation and prospects
.
Neuropsychology
.
2023
;
37
:
673
682
.

12

Jack
 
CR
,
Bennett
 
DA
,
Blennow
 
K
, et al.  
NIA-AA Research framework: Toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
2018
;
14
:
535
562
.

13

Jack
 
CR
,
Knopman
 
DS
,
Jagust
 
WJ
, et al.  
Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: An updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers
.
Lancet Neurol
.
2013
;
12
:
207
216
.

14

Zetterberg
 
H
,
Bendlin
 
BB
.
Biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease—preparing for a new era of disease-modifying therapies
.
Mol Psychiatry
.
2021
;
26
:
296
308
.

15

Liu
 
CC
,
Kanekiyo
 
T
,
Xu
 
H
,
Bu
 
G
.
Apolipoprotein E and Alzheimer disease: Risk, mechanisms and therapy
.
Nat Rev Neurol
.
2013
;
9
:
106
118
.

16

Jessen
 
F
,
Amariglio
 
RE
,
van Boxtel
 
M
, et al.  
A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
2014
;
10
:
844
852
.

17

Zimmermann
 
JF
,
Butler
 
CR
.
Accelerated long-term forgetting in asymptomatic APOE ε4 carriers
.
Lancet Neurol
.
2018
;
17
:
394
395
.

18

Tort-Merino
 
A
,
Laine
 
M
,
Valech
 
N
, et al.  
Accelerated long-term forgetting over three months in asymptomatic APOE ɛ4 carriers
.
Ann Clin Transl Neurol
.
2021
;
8
:
477
484
.

19

Van Der Werf
 
SP
,
Geurts
 
S
,
De Werd
 
MME
.
Subjective memory ability and long-term forgetting in patients referred for neuropsychological assessment
.
Front Psychol
.
2016
;
7
:
605
.

20

Tort-Merino
 
A
,
Valech
 
N
,
Laine
 
M
, et al.  
Accelerated long-term forgetting in individuals with subjective cognitive decline and amyloid-β positivity
.
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
.
2021
;
36
:
1037
1049
.

21

Manes
 
F
,
Serrano
 
C
,
Calcagno
 
ML
,
Cardozo
 
J
,
Hodges
 
J
.
Accelerated forgetting in subjects with memory complaints: A new form of mild cognitive impairment?
 
J Neurol
.
2008
;
255
:
1067
1070
.

22

Wearn
 
AR
,
Saunders-Jennings
 
E
,
Nurdal
 
V
, et al.  
Accelerated long-term forgetting in healthy older adults predicts cognitive decline over 1 year
.
Alzheimers Res Ther
.
2020
;
12
:
119
.

23

Weston
 
PSJ
,
Nicholas
 
JM
,
Henley
 
SMD
, et al.  
Accelerated long-term forgetting in presymptomatic autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease: A cross-sectional study
.
Lancet Neurol
.
2018
;
17
:
123
132
.

24

Yang
 
J
,
Kong
 
C
,
Jia
 
L
, et al.  
Association of accelerated long-term forgetting and senescence-related blood-borne factors in asymptomatic individuals from families with autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease
.
Alzheimers Res Ther
.
2021
;
13
:
107
.

25

O’Connor
 
A
,
Weston
 
PSJ
,
Pavisic
 
IM
, et al.  
Quantitative detection and staging of presymptomatic cognitive decline in familial Alzheimer’s disease: A retrospective cohort analysis
.
Alzheimers Res Ther
.
2020
;
12
:
126
.

26

Sacripante
 
R
,
Logie
 
RH
,
Baddeley
 
A
,
Della Salla
 
S
.
Forgetting rates of gist and peripheral episodic details in prose recall
.
Mem Cognit
.
2023
;
51
:
71
86
.

27

Lane
 
CA
,
Parker
 
TD
,
Cash
 
DM
, et al.  
Study protocol: Insight 46—A neuroscience sub-study of the MRC national survey of health and development
.
BMC Neurol
.
2017
;
17:
75
.

28

Kuh
 
D
,
Wong
 
A
,
Shah
 
I
, et al.  
The MRC national survey of health and development reaches age 70: Maintaining participation at older ages in a birth cohort study
.
Eur J Epidemiol
.
2016
;
31
:
1135
1147
.

29

James
 
SN
,
Lane
 
CA
,
Parker
 
TD
, et al.  
Using a birth cohort to study brain health and preclinical dementia: Recruitment and participation rates in Insight 46
.
BMC Res Notes
.
2018
;
11
:
885
.

30

Keshavan
 
A
,
Pannee
 
J
,
Karikari
 
TK
, et al.  
Population-based blood screening for preclinical Alzheimer’s disease in a British birth cohort at age 70
.
Brain
.
2020
;
144
:
434
449
.

31

Keuss
 
SE
,
Coath
 
W
,
Nicholas
 
JM
, et al.  
Associations of β-amyloid and vascular burden with rates of neurodegeneration in cognitively normal members of the 1946 British birth cohort
.
Neurology
.
2022
;
99
:
e129
e141
.

32

Lu
 
K
,
Nicholas
 
JM
,
Collins
 
JD
, et al.  
Cognition at age 70: Life course predictors and associations with brain pathologies
.
Neurology
.
2019
;
93
:
e2144
e2156
.

33

Lu
 
K
,
Nicholas
 
JM
,
James
 
S-N
, et al.  
Increased variability in reaction time is associated with amyloid beta pathology at age 70
.
Alzheimer’s Dement (Amst)
.
2020
;
12
:
e12076
.

34

Lu
 
K
,
Nicholas
 
JM
,
Weston
 
PSJ
, et al.  
Visuomotor integration deficits are common to both familial and sporadic preclinical Alzheimer’s disease
.
Brain Commun
.
2021
;
3
:
fcab003
. doi:

35

Lu
 
K
,
Nicholas
 
JM
,
Pertzov
 
Y
, et al.  
Dissociable effects of APOE-ε4 and β-amyloid pathology on visual working memory
.
Nat Aging
.
2021
;
1
:
1002
1009
.

36

Papp
 
KV
,
Amariglio
 
RE
,
Dekhtyar
 
M
, et al.  
Development of a psychometrically equivalent short form of the Face-Name Associative Memory Exam for use along the early Alzheimer’s disease trajectory
.
Clin Neuropsychol
.
2014
;
28
:
771
785
.

37

Coughlin
 
AK
.
The adult memory and information processing battery (AMIPB): Test manual
.
Psychology Department, St James’ Hospital
;
1985
.

38

Zhang
 
X
,
Lv
 
L
,
Min
 
G
,
Wang
 
Q
,
Zhao
 
Y
,
Li
 
Y
.
Overview of the complex figure test and its clinical application in neuropsychiatric disorders, including copying and recall
.
Front Neurol
.
2021
;
12
:
680474
.

39

Allen
 
L
,
Brechin
 
D
,
Skilbeck
 
C
,
Fox
 
R
.
The figure copy and recall test of the adult memory and information processing battery: Inter-rater reliability
.
Br J Clin Psychol
.
2007
;
46
(
Pt 2
):
241
245
.

40

Lu
 
K
,
Pavisic
 
IM
,
James
 
S
, et al.  
Accelerated forgetting is sensitive to β-amyloid pathology and cerebral atrophy in cognitively normal 72-year-olds
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
2020
;
16
(
S6
):
e040987
.

41

Muhlert
 
N
,
Milton
 
F
,
Butler
 
CR
,
Kapur
 
N
,
Zeman
 
AZ
.
Accelerated forgetting of real-life events in transient epileptic amnesia
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2010
;
48
:
3235
3244
.

42

Coath
 
W
,
Modat
 
M
,
Cardoso
 
MJ
, et al.  
Operationalizing the centiloid scale for [18 F]florbetapir PET studies on PET/MRI
.
Alzheimer’s Dement (Amst)
.
2023
;
15
:
e12434
.

43

Freeborough
 
PA
,
Fox
 
NC
.
The boundary shift integral: An accurate and robust measure of cerebral volume changes from registered repeat MRI
.
IEEE Trans Med Imaging
.
1997
;
16
:
623
629
.

44

Leung
 
KK
,
Barnes
 
J
,
Ridgway
 
GR
, et al.  
Automated cross-sectional and longitudinal hippocampal volume measurement in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease
.
Neuroimage
.
2010
;
51
:
1345
1359
.

45

Prins
 
ND
,
Scheltens
 
P
.
White matter hyperintensities, cognitive impairment and dementia: An update
.
Nat Rev Neurol
.
2015
;
11
:
157
165
.

46

Sudre
 
CH
,
Cardoso
 
MJ
,
Bouvy
 
WH
,
Biessels
 
GJ
,
Barnes
 
J
,
Ourselin
 
S
.
Bayesian model selection for pathological neuroimaging data applied to white matter lesion segmentation
.
IEEE Trans Med Imaging
.
2015
;
34
:
2079
2102
.

47

Malone
 
IB
,
Leung
 
KK
,
Clegg
 
S
, et al.  
Accurate automatic estimation of total intracranial volume: A nuisance variable with less nuisance
.
Neuroimage
.
2015
;
104
:
366
372
.

48

Rami
 
L
,
Mollica
 
M
,
García-Sanchez
 
C
, et al.  
The subjective cognitive decline questionnaire (SCD-Q): A validation study
.
J Alzheimers Dis
.
2014
;
41
:
453
466
.

49

Pavisic
 
IM
,
Lu
 
K
,
Keuss
 
SE
, et al.  
Subjective cognitive complaints at age 70: Associations with amyloid and mental health
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
.
2021
;
92
:
1215
1221
.

50

Spielberger
 
CD
,
Gorsuch
 
RL
,
Lushene
 
R
,
Vagg
 
PR
,
Jacobs
 
GA
.
Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory
.
Consulting Psychologists Press
;
1983
.

51

Richards
 
M
,
James
 
SN
,
Sizer
 
A
, et al.  
Identifying the lifetime cognitive and socioeconomic antecedents of cognitive state: Seven decades of follow-up in a British birth cohort study
.
BMJ Open
.
2019
;
9
:
e024404
.

52

Rentz
 
DM
,
Amariglio
 
RE
,
Becker
 
JA
, et al.  
Face-name associative memory performance is related to amyloid burden in normal elderly
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2011
;
49
:
2776
2783
.

53

Sanabria
 
A
,
Alegret
 
M
,
Rodriguez-Gomez
 
O
, et al.  
The Spanish version of face-name associative memory exam (S-FNAME) performance is related to amyloid burden in subjective cognitive decline
.
Sci Rep
.
2018
;
8
:
3828
.

54

Mayes
 
A
,
Montaldi
 
D
,
Migo
 
E
.
Associative memory and the medial temporal lobes
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
2007
;
11
:
126
135
.

55

Sperling
 
R
,
Chua
 
E
,
Cocchiarella
 
A
, et al.  
Putting names to faces: Successful encoding of associative memories activates the anterior hippocampal formation
.
Neuroimage
.
2003
;
20
:
1400
1410
.

56

Putcha
 
D
,
O’Keefe
 
K
,
LaViolette
 
P
, et al.  
Reliability of functional magnetic resonance imaging associative encoding memory paradigms in non-demented elderly adults
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2011
;
32
:
2027
2044
.

57

Barnes
 
J
,
Bartlett
 
JW
,
Van De Pol
 
LA
, et al.  
A meta-analysis of hippocampal atrophy rates in Alzheimer’s disease
.
Neurobiol Aging
.
2009
;
30
:
1711
1723
.

58

Xie
 
J
,
Brayne
 
C
,
Matthews
 
FE
.
Survival times in people with dementia: Analysis from population based cohort study with 14 year follow-up
.
BMJ
.
2008
;
336
:
258
262
.

59

Dubravac
 
M
,
Meier
 
B
.
Cognitive load at encoding hurts memory selectivity
.
Q J Exp Psychol.
 
2022
;
76
:
1515
1538
.

60

Rashal
 
E
,
Yeshurun
 
Y
,
Kimchi
 
R
.
Attentional requirements in perceptual grouping depend on the processes involved in the organization
.
Atten Percept Psychophys
.
2017
;
79
:
2073
2087
.

61

Kim
 
MS
,
Namgoong
 
Y
,
Youn
 
T
.
Effect of organizational strategy on visual memory in patients with schizophrenia
.
Psychiatry Clin Neurosci
.
2008
;
62
:
427
434
.

62

Mayes
 
AR
,
Hunkin
 
NM
,
Isaac
 
C
,
Muhlert
 
N
.
Are there distinct forms of accelerated forgetting and, if so, why?
 
Cortex
.
2019
;
110
:
115
126
.

63

Cassel
 
A
,
Kopelman
 
MD
.
Have we forgotten about forgetting? A critical review of ‘accelerated long-term forgetting’ in temporal lobe epilepsy
.
Cortex
.
2019
;
110
:
141
149
.

64

Altuna
 
M
,
Olmedo-Saura
 
G
,
Carmona-Iragui
 
M
,
Fortea
 
J
.
Mechanisms involved in epileptogenesis in Alzheimer’s disease and their therapeutic implications
.
Int J Mol Sci
.
2022
;
23
:
4307
.

65

Harris
 
SS
,
Wolf
 
F
,
Strooper
 
D
,
Busche
 
MA
.
Tipping the scales: Peptide-dependent dysregulation of neural circuit dynamics in Alzheimer’s disease
.
Neuron
.
2020
;
107
:
417
435
.

66

Busche
 
MA
,
Chen
 
X
,
Henning
 
HA
, et al.  
Critical role of soluble amyloid-β for early hippocampal hyperactivity in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
.
2012
;
109
:
8740
8745
.

67

Xie
 
H
,
Hou
 
S
,
Jiang
 
J
,
Sekutowicz
 
M
,
Kelly
 
J
,
Bacskai
 
BJ
.
Rapid cell death is preceded by amyloid plaque-mediated oxidative stress
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
.
2013
;
110
:
7904
7909
.

68

Butler
 
CR
,
Bhaduri
 
A
,
Acosta-Cabronero
 
J
, et al.  
Transient epileptic amnesia: Regional brain atrophy and its relationship to memory deficits
.
Brain
.
2009
;
132
:
357
368
.

69

Cassel
 
A
,
Morris
 
R
,
Koutroumanidis
 
M
,
Kopelman
 
M
.
Forgetting in temporal lobe epilepsy: When does it become accelerated?
 
Cortex
.
2016
;
78
:
70
84
.

70

Baddeley
 
A
,
Atkinson
 
A
,
Kemp
 
S
,
Allen
 
R
.
The problem of detecting long-term forgetting: Evidence from the crimes test and the four doors test
.
Cortex
.
2019
;
110
:
69
79
.

71

Wilkinson
 
H
,
Holdstock
 
JS
,
Baker
 
G
,
Herbert
 
A
,
Clague
 
F
,
Downes
 
JJ
.
Long-term accelerated forgetting of verbal and non-verbal information in temporal lobe epilepsy
.
Cortex
.
2012
;
48
:
317
332
.

72

Kern
 
S
,
Zetterberg
 
H
,
Kern
 
J
, et al.  
Prevalence of preclinical Alzheimer disease: Comparison of current classification systems
.
Neurology
.
2018
;
90
:
e1682
e1691
.

73

Jansen
 
WJ
,
Ossenkoppele
 
R
,
Knol
 
DL
, et al.  
Prevalence of cerebral amyloid pathology in persons without dementia: A meta-analysis
.
JAMA
.
2015
;
313
:
1924
1938
.

74

Cho
 
H
,
Choi
 
JY
,
Hwang
 
MS
, et al.  
Tau PET in Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment
.
Neurology
.
2016
;
87
:
375
383
.

75

Buckley
 
RF
,
Hanseeuw
 
B
,
Schultz
 
AP
, et al.  
Region-specific association of subjective cognitive decline with tauopathy independent of global β-amyloid burden
.
JAMA Neurol
.
2017
;
74
:
1455
1463
.

Author notes

Sebastian J Crutch and Jonathan M Schott contributed equally to this work.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact [email protected] for reprints and translation rights for reprints. All other permissions can be obtained through our RightsLink service via the Permissions link on the article page on our site—for further information please contact [email protected].

Supplementary data