Abstract

The thalamus has complex connections with the cortex and is involved in various cognitive processes. Despite increasing interest in the thalamus and the underlying thalamo-cortical interaction, little is known about thalamo-cortical connections after paediatric arterial ischaemic stroke. Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate thalamo-cortical connections and their association with cognitive performance after arterial ischaemic stroke. Twenty patients in the chronic phase after paediatric arterial ischaemic stroke (≥2 years after diagnosis, diagnosed <16 years; aged 5–23 years, mean: 15.1 years) and 20 healthy controls matched for age and sex were examined in a cross-sectional study design. Cognitive performance (selective attention, inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility) was evaluated using standardized neuropsychological tests. Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to examine functional thalamo-cortical connectivity. Lesion masks were integrated in the preprocessing pipeline to ensure that structurally damaged voxels did not influence functional connectivity analyses. Cognitive performance (selective attention, inhibition, and working memory) was significantly reduced in patients compared to controls. Network analyses revealed significantly lower thalamo-cortical connectivity for the motor, auditory, visual, default mode network, salience, left/right executive, and dorsal attention network in patients compared with controls. Interestingly, analyses additionally revealed higher thalamo-cortical connectivity in some subdivisions of the thalamus for the default mode network (medial nuclei), motor (lateral nuclei), dorsal attention (anterior nuclei), and the left executive network (posterior nuclei) in patients compared with controls. Increased and decreased thalamo-cortical connectivity strength within the same networks was, however, found in different thalamic subdivisions. Thus, alterations in thalamo-cortical connectivity strength after paediatric stroke seem to point in both directions, with stronger as well as weaker thalamo-cortical connectivity in patients compared with controls. Multivariate linear regression, with lesion size and age as covariates, revealed significant correlations between cognitive performance (selective attention, inhibition, and working memory) and the strength of thalamo-cortical connectivity in the motor, auditory, visual, default mode network, posterior default mode network, salience, left/right executive, and dorsal attention network after childhood stroke. Our data suggest that the interaction between different sub-nuclei of the thalamus and several cortical networks relates to post-stroke cognition. The variability in cognitive outcomes after paediatric stroke might partly be explained by functional thalamo-cortical connectivity strength.

Introduction

Paediatric arterial ischaemic stroke (AIS) occurs rarely, but two-thirds of survivors may have lifelong neurological, motor, and cognitive deficits,1–3 which in turn can lead to reduced health-related quality of life.4,5 Post-stroke outcome varies with patient-related factors (e.g. age at stroke and socio-economic status) and stroke- and lesion-related factors, including stroke severity, lesion size, and lesion location.1,3,6 However, these factors have only limited predictive power for long-term cognitive outcome.7 Many studies have observed that initial stroke severity and lesion volume fail to explain most of the variability in cognitive outcome.7 A possible reason is that, although AIS induces focal structural lesions, widespread alterations in functional connectivity networks occur and may further influence cognitive outcome and recovery. Consequently, alterations in large-scale functional brain networks rather than in single brain regions are thought to determine post-stroke cognitive outcome.8,9

The thalamus presents complex cortical and subcortical connections and is described to be a critical integrative node in large-scale functional brain networks.10–14 The thalamo-cortical loop develops before birth and plays a crucial role in the regulation of the development of cortical and thalamic territories.15,16 Different nuclei of the thalamus have been associated with specific cognitive functions, such as processing speed, attention, and executive functions.12,14,17–19 Hence, the thalamus and especially its connections are thought to play a crucial role in cognitive recovery after stroke.20

Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) can be used to characterize large-scale systems such as the thalamo-cortical networks19,21–24 and measures the temporal correlation of blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signals between different regions in the resting brain.25 This method has gained wide appeal owing to its simple application and reliability within and between individuals.26,27 Various resting-state networks can be drawn from a single scan while the child is at rest. This task-free procedure is particularly valuable in paediatric samples. Further, the investigation of resting-state networks has been shown to be of great clinical value, providing sensitive markers of disease.28

Studies in adult patients after stroke tend to agree that the strength of resting-state functional connectivity is associated with cognitive functions, including language, memory, executive functions, and attention.29–33 This is supported by findings of longitudinal studies revealing increases of functional connectivity over time which are associated with recovery of motor function.34–36 Only a few studies have investigated resting-state networks after AIS in a paediatric population.8,9,37–39 Our recent study37 and findings from another study9 have shown that the connectivity within the default mode network (DMN) and executive network is reduced in paediatric patients after AIS compared with controls, which in turn was correlated with reduced cognitive performance. Further, patients with hemiparesis after paediatric AIS showed reduced interhemispheric connectivity strength in the motor network compared to patients without hemiparesis and healthy controls.40 In studies that compared healthy controls to children with perinatal AIS, both weaker and stronger functional connectivity of the primary motor cortex to other brain regions was found.38

Functional connectivity measures between cortical and subcortical structures also predicted motor outcome after perinatal stroke.41 Altered thalamic volume, particularly in the non-lesioned hemisphere, has been associated with motor function,42 suggesting an important role for the thalamus in the developmental plasticity that determines motor function after perinatal stroke.

In a previous study, we demonstrated that the thalamus has functional connections to multiple cortical resting-state networks in healthy children, adolescents, and young adults and that stronger thalamo-cortical connectivity was associated with better cognitive performance in the healthy development between 5 and 25 years of age.19 However, the association between resting-state thalamo-cortical connectivity and cognition has never been investigated in patients after paediatric AIS. To this end, we aimed to investigate functional connectivity of the thalamo-cortical system and cognitive performance (including selective attention and executive functions) in patients after unilateral paediatric AIS and in healthy controls. Based on the literature presented previously, we hypothesized that (i) thalamo-cortical connectivity is reduced in patients after stroke compared with controls and that (ii) stronger thalamo-cortical connectivity is related to better cognitive performance (selective attention and executive functions).

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants were recruited as part of the HERO study43 examining functional reorganization after childhood stroke in a cross-sectional study design. The HERO study has been reviewed and approved by the Local Ethics Committee Bern (212/13), Switzerland.

Patients were identified by the Swiss Neuropaediatric Stroke Registry (SNPSR)—a multicentre, prospective, and population-based registry that includes children diagnosed with unilateral AIS under the age of 16 years.44 Patients met the following inclusion criteria: diagnosis of AIS (confirmed by MRI or CT) before the age of 16 years and at least 2 years prior to recruitment and older than 5 years of age at time of assessment to enable adequate compliance. The gap of 2 years between the AIS diagnosis and study examination was chosen to ensure that the patients were in the chronic stage, as the critical time window for recovery can extend beyond 1 year post-stroke.45 Exclusion criteria were active epilepsy (defined as seizures or treatment with anti-epileptic drugs during the 12 months prior to study participation), iron implants, claustrophobia, and behavioural problems that make an MRI scan impossible.

The control group, a sample of typically developing peers comparable in age and sex to the patient group, was recruited through advertisements on the hospital intranet and flyers. All healthy participants fulfilled the following inclusion criteria: (i) absence of neurological disease or psychiatric disorders, (ii) intelligence above IQ >85, and (iii) no contraindications for MRI (metal braces or metallic implants). All children and adolescents or their legal representative signed a written consent to participate in the study. Consent was obtained according to the Declaration of Helsinki. All examinations were performed at the University Hospital Bern, Inselspital, Switzerland. For more detailed information on the study design, see the previously published study protocol.43

Of the 29 patients recruited in the HERO study, 20 were included in the present analysis. Nine patients had to be excluded due to bilateral lesions (n = 4), retainer artefacts (n = 1), error in T1-weighted anatomical image or BOLD sequences (n = 2), or developmental delay or behavioural problems impeding compliance during the assessments (n = 2). Detailed clinical characteristics of the study participants are provided in Supplementary Table 1.

Cognitive assessment

Cognitive functions were assessed with standardized neuropsychological tests conducted by trained psychologists or by study assistants and postgraduates (under supervision). We only included tasks with no fine motor components, as some patients had residual symptoms of hemiparesis. This ensured that reduced motor abilities did not influence cognitive performance. Neuropsychological assessments was performed at the Division of Neuropaediatrics, Development and Rehabilitation at the University Hospital, Inselspital, Bern, Switzerland, and were conducted within the same week as the MRI appointment at the Institute of Diagnostic and Interventional Neuroradiology, Inselspital, Bern, Switzerland.

To determine cognitive outcome, selective attention and executive functions were assessed: (i) selective attention was assessed with the cancellation subtest of the the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, which is a visual-spatial search task. The three core dimensions of executive functions were also assessed:46 (ii) Working memory [spatial positioning subtest of the Learning and Memory Test (basic-MLT);47 (iii) inhibition and (iv) cognitive flexibility (colour–word inference test of the Delis Kaplan Executive Function System48). This test was conducted only with participants older than 8 years. This task includes four conditions where participants name coloured squares (condition 1), read words indicating colours printed in black ink (condition 2), name the incongruent ink colour of printed colour words (condition 3; inhibition) and switch between naming the ink colour and reading the colour words (condition 4; cognitive flexibility). This study contains the results from condition 3 (inhibition) which requires participants to inhibit a learned response (i.e. naming the colour of the words while ignoring their semantic content). Furthermore, this study contains the results of condition 4 (cognitive flexibility), where participants have to inhibit an overlearned response (i.e. naming colour of the words while ignoring their semantic content) but on top requires participants to demonstrate flexibility by set shifting. For more detailed information about neuropsychological tests see the previously published study protocol.43 Raw scores were used for analyses of functional connectivity measures, so that age could be fitted in the model. Each raw score was converted into a Z-score. To test whether cognitive performance differed between groups, scaled scores were analyzed with paired t-tests.

MRI image acquisition and preprocessing

All participants were told to stay awake with their eyes closed and to remain as motionless as possible during the MRI scan. A head support system consisting of two pillows positioned on both sides of the head was used to minimize head motion. Earplugs were given to the participants to minimize the scanner noise.

Structural images

MRI data were acquired on a 3 T Magnetom Verio scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). The structural images were aquired using a three-dimensional magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo T1-weighted sequence [repetition time (TR) = 2530 ms, echo time (TE) = 2.96 ms, inversion time = 1100 ms, and a flip angle (FA) = 7°, field of view (FOV) 256×256 mm2, matrix dimension 256×256, leading to an isovoxel resolution of 1×1×1 mm3, acquisition time (TA) = 5.05 min).

Lesion-related characteristics were determined by a board-certified neuroradiologist (N.S.). Lesion laterality was classified depending on the affected hemisphere (left, right or bilateral) and lesion location was divided into three categories (cortical, subcortical or combined cortical and subcortical, according to this previous work49). Ischaemic lesions were manually segmented (slice by slice) on the structural images (T1) to calculate the volume of the affected brain tissue using the open-source software 3D Slicer, version 4 (https://www.slicer.org). Affected brain tissue was selected based on hypointensity on the T1-weighted images. Both CSF filled areas as well as encephalomalacic areas and gliotic parts may appear hypo-intense on T1-weighted as well. Only post-ischaemic defects were evaluated and the adjacent CSF spaces were not included in the analysis. Lesion size was defined as the affected brain tissue in relation to the total brain volume. Total brain volume was calculated using the MATLAB-based toolbox Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12). More in detail, the structural images were then transformed into the standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template and then segmented into grey matter, white matter, and CSF. In the processing steps for segmentation within SPM12, there are efforts to overcome problems of gliotic parts (e.g. as may be originated by lacune) that eventually would be associated to CSF tissue class. Notably, SPM12 is widely used for segmentation of anatomical images.50,51 Furthermore, the segmented lesions were integrated as masks in the preprocessing pipeline. Resting-state connectivity was only measured in healthy tissue.

Functional images

Functional images were acquired using a T2*-weighted multi-band, simultaneous excitation echo planar imaging (mb-EPI) sequence (TR = 300 ms, TE = 30 ms, FA = 30°, FOV 230×230 mm2, matrix dimension 64×64, 32 axial slices positioned along the anterior commissure and the posterior commissure with a slice thickness of 3.6 mm, gap 0 mm, leading to an isovoxel resolution of 3.6×3.6×3.6 mm3). The TA = 5.06 min, and each scan consisted of 1000 image volumes.

Preprocessing

Preprocessing of fMRI data was performed using FMRIB Software Library (FSL; http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl52–54). The fMRI time series for each subject was pre-processed as follows (pipeline recommended elsewhere55,56): (i) rigid body realignment; (ii) within-subject intensity normalization (in which the intensity across all voxels was scaled to a norm value of 1000. The BOLD signal is then represented in a mode 1000 scale (10 units = 1% BOLD); (iii) coregistration of functional images to each of the individual anatomical images. All functional images were resampled to an isovoxel space with 3 mm × 3 mm × 3 mm spatial resolution; (iv) finally, functional images were transformed to the standard MNI template.

Identification and treatment of motion artefacts by censoring functional MRI time series

Head motion could compromise the computation of correlations in resting-state functional connectivity fMRI. Steps to identify and eventually remove motion artefacts during fMRI acquisition were therefore taken.55,56 Framewise displacement (FD) was used to record the absolute amount of motion during scanning and the relative measure expressed as root mean square value of the differentiated BOLD time series (by backward differences, DVARS). This DVARS measure captures the change in signal intensity from one volume to the adjacent previous volume.

With these two measures, each individual fMRI time series was censored (i.e. scrubbing). The following criteria were used to identify and quantify ‘artefact-affected volumes’: FD > 0.2 and DVARS > 0.38, i.e. a ‘geometrical’ and a ‘physiological’ surrogate for the motion-related signal component. The detailed procedure of censoring volumes and its effect on the quality of the data of each participant are defined in more detail in the Supplementary Materials (Supplementary Fig. 1).

Nuisance regressors

A set of six motion estimates [R = (x, y, z, pitch, yaw, roll)] were stored for each subject during the realignment step. These motion estimates were the basis for the computation of three additional motion-related indices: their squares (R2), (Rt—1) and (R2t—1), where t and t—1 apply to the current and immediately preceding time point of the fMRI time series.

In summary, this procedure results in 24 motion-related indices that were further used as nuisance regressors in the multiple regression analyses. Tissue-based signals from average signal across voxels within a ventricular mask for the CSF signal and white matter mask for the white matter signal were also used as nuisance regressors. Finally, global signal regression57,58 was included as an effective procedure to remove motion-related artefacts.55 A set of 27 nuisance regressors were included in the multiple regressions.

Processing of neuroimaging data and statistical analyses

To define a mask for the thalamus (region 10 and 49; left and right thalamus) the Harvard–Oxford cortical and subcortical structural atlases were adopted (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/Atlases). In total, 672 voxels comprised in the final thalamus mask, which was down-sampled into matching 3×3×3 mm3 in MNI space.

Identifying thalamic connectivity patterns

To identify thalamic connectivity patterns, the same strategy as in previous work was adopted.19,22 First, functional images from patients with lesions in the right hemisphere (n = 4) were flipped along the midsagittal plane, so that the affected hemisphere corresponded to the left hemisphere in the whole patient sample. Then, time series from all voxels of the thalamus were correlated with all voxels of the rest of the brain using Pearson’s correlation coefficient. Six hundred and seventy-two seed-based correlation maps (connectivity maps) for each participant resulted from this procedure. All correlation maps were then Fisher-transformed to Z-scores [Z = 0.5 * ln (1 + r/1−r)]. And concatenated into a single 4D data set with the dimensions of 61×73×61×26 880 voxels (a single volume has 61×73×61 voxels; 672 volumes for each of the 40 participants). Then, the independent component analyses (ICAs) were calculated on the resulting seed-based correlation maps using FSL’s Multivariate Exploratory Linear Optimized Decomposition into Independent Components to identify spatially distinct connectivity maps. The resulting 4D data set was decomposed into 20 spatially independent components (ICs).22 From this set of ICs, we were able to identify nine well-known brain networks for further analysis.

Thalamo-cortical connectivity and statistical analysis

To quantify the functional connectivity between each thalamic voxel and brain network, we first determined the contributions of each IC network with the individual thalamic connectivity maps (subject level). These individual thalamic connectivity maps and the IC network maps were then transformed into vectors, and linear regression was used to quantify functional connectivity for each voxel across all individuals. In particular, linear regression was used to measure the contribution from each IC to the voxel’s seed-based correlation map (for each given voxel in the thalamus). The regression analysis was performed for all of the 672 voxels in the thalamus, and nine beta values were obtained for each voxel. A voxel-wise, one-sample t-test was performed on every network’s thalamic beta map across subjects to find all significant clusters in the thalamus for each network separately.

Potential differences between the groups for the ICA components produced were assessed using paired t-tests. To examine the impact of cognitive functions (processing speed, selective attention, inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility) on thalamo-cortical connectivity strength in patients, we applied multivariate linear regressions. Cognitive variables were integrated as independent variables and connectivity strength values as dependent variables, with age and lesion size as covariates.

We applied false discovery rate (FDR)-adjusted P-values to correct for multiple testing. An FDR-adjusted P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. All analysis steps are similar to the procedure used previously in an adult population22 and in a paediatric population.19

The significant clusters in the thalamus were identified and labelled according to the thalamus atlas of Morel and Krauth.59,60 This atlas is built in MNI space and includes 40 small thalamic nuclei. Analyses were performed with MATLAB 9.2 (MathWorks, Sherborn, MA, USA). We focused mainly on the largest of the 40 nuclei present in the Morel atlas without differentiating finer subdivisions of the different nuclei to minimize identification errors.

Data availability

Individual patient data, including neuroimaging data, cognitive data, and some demographical variables are available upon reasonable request after signing a confidentiality statement and a data sharing agreement and in accordance with data privacy statements signed by all patients.

Results

Descriptive and cognitive measures

Details of the clinical characteristics of the study participants are provided in Supplementary Table 1. Patients and healthy controls did not differ in terms of age and sex, as groups were matched for these variables (Table 1). Patients after paediatric stroke had significantly lower scores for selective attention [t(38) = 2.05, P = 0.048], working memory [t(38) = 2.51, P = 0.016], and inhibition [t(38) = 2.18, P = 0.036] than controls. There was no significant between-group difference in cognitive flexibility (Table 1). However, there is a trend, with a moderate effect size, for cognitive flexibility to be lower in children after stroke.

Table 1

Demographic and cognitive variables of patients and healthy controls

PatientsControlst(df)pCohen’s d
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)
Sex, n (%)
 Female8 (40.0)8 (40.0)
 Male12 (60.0)12 (60.0)
Age at assessment (years)15.01 (4.28)15.2 (4.10)0.14 (39)0.900
Selective attention (SS)8.09 (3.30)11.58 (2.58)2.05 (39)0.048*0.64
 Range2–136–16
Working memory (PR)43.37 (31.84)69.68 (29.21)2.51 (37)0.016*0.79
 Range3–1008–99
Inhibition (SS)9.32 (3.11)11.21 (2.18)2.18 (37)0.036*0.71
 Range2–136–14
Cognitive flexibility (SS)9.63 (3.58)11.58 (2.29)2.00 (37)0.0520.65
 Range3–138–15
PatientsControlst(df)pCohen’s d
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)
Sex, n (%)
 Female8 (40.0)8 (40.0)
 Male12 (60.0)12 (60.0)
Age at assessment (years)15.01 (4.28)15.2 (4.10)0.14 (39)0.900
Selective attention (SS)8.09 (3.30)11.58 (2.58)2.05 (39)0.048*0.64
 Range2–136–16
Working memory (PR)43.37 (31.84)69.68 (29.21)2.51 (37)0.016*0.79
 Range3–1008–99
Inhibition (SS)9.32 (3.11)11.21 (2.18)2.18 (37)0.036*0.71
 Range2–136–14
Cognitive flexibility (SS)9.63 (3.58)11.58 (2.29)2.00 (37)0.0520.65
 Range3–138–15

SD, standard deviation; SS, scaled score; PR, percent range, * p < 0.05

Table 1

Demographic and cognitive variables of patients and healthy controls

PatientsControlst(df)pCohen’s d
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)
Sex, n (%)
 Female8 (40.0)8 (40.0)
 Male12 (60.0)12 (60.0)
Age at assessment (years)15.01 (4.28)15.2 (4.10)0.14 (39)0.900
Selective attention (SS)8.09 (3.30)11.58 (2.58)2.05 (39)0.048*0.64
 Range2–136–16
Working memory (PR)43.37 (31.84)69.68 (29.21)2.51 (37)0.016*0.79
 Range3–1008–99
Inhibition (SS)9.32 (3.11)11.21 (2.18)2.18 (37)0.036*0.71
 Range2–136–14
Cognitive flexibility (SS)9.63 (3.58)11.58 (2.29)2.00 (37)0.0520.65
 Range3–138–15
PatientsControlst(df)pCohen’s d
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)
Sex, n (%)
 Female8 (40.0)8 (40.0)
 Male12 (60.0)12 (60.0)
Age at assessment (years)15.01 (4.28)15.2 (4.10)0.14 (39)0.900
Selective attention (SS)8.09 (3.30)11.58 (2.58)2.05 (39)0.048*0.64
 Range2–136–16
Working memory (PR)43.37 (31.84)69.68 (29.21)2.51 (37)0.016*0.79
 Range3–1008–99
Inhibition (SS)9.32 (3.11)11.21 (2.18)2.18 (37)0.036*0.71
 Range2–136–14
Cognitive flexibility (SS)9.63 (3.58)11.58 (2.29)2.00 (37)0.0520.65
 Range3–138–15

SD, standard deviation; SS, scaled score; PR, percent range, * p < 0.05

Thalamo-cortical networks

Analyses revealed nine networks involved in cognitive, sensory-, and motor-related processing. These networks were comparable with the networks that have previously been observed in rs-fMRI studies in adults22 and children.19,22,26,27,61 These nine networks represent the DMN, the posterior DMN, left and right executive, auditory, dorsal attention, motor, salience, and lateral visual networks (Fig. 1).

Group differences in thalamo-cortical networks. Nine cortical networks from group-level ICAs (first and third row) and thalamic clusters that differed between groups for each resting-sate network (second and fourth row) are depicted. (i) Motor network; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the mediodorsal nucleus and nuclei from the anterior group and higher thalamo-cortical connectivity in nuclei from the lateral group. (ii) Visual network; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the pulvinar, ventral lateral, ventral posterior, mediodorsal and lateral posterior nuclei. (iii) Auditory network; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the lateral and medial nuclei, including the lateral dorsal and mediodorsal nuclei. (iv) DMN; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the pulvinar and higher thalamo-cortical connectivity in the mediodorsal nucleus. (v) Posterior DMN; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the lateral nuclei and in the pulvinar. (vi) Salience network; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the mediodorsal nucleus and the pulvinar. (vii) Dorsal attention network; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the pulvinar, and mediodorsal nucleus and higher thalamo-cortical connectivity in the anteroventral nucleus. (viii) Right and left executive network; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the mediodorsal nucleus, pulvinar and in the lateral group. (ix) Left executive network: patients showed higher thalamo-cortical connectivity in the pulvinar. Notes. DMN, default mode network; L-EF, left executive network; R-EF, right executive network. The right side of the brain is on the right side of the image. All sub-regions of the thalamus are thresholded at FDR-corrected P = 0.05. Only FDR-corrected voxels are depicted in the Figure.
Figure 1

Group differences in thalamo-cortical networks. Nine cortical networks from group-level ICAs (first and third row) and thalamic clusters that differed between groups for each resting-sate network (second and fourth row) are depicted. (i) Motor network; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the mediodorsal nucleus and nuclei from the anterior group and higher thalamo-cortical connectivity in nuclei from the lateral group. (ii) Visual network; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the pulvinar, ventral lateral, ventral posterior, mediodorsal and lateral posterior nuclei. (iii) Auditory network; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the lateral and medial nuclei, including the lateral dorsal and mediodorsal nuclei. (iv) DMN; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the pulvinar and higher thalamo-cortical connectivity in the mediodorsal nucleus. (v) Posterior DMN; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the lateral nuclei and in the pulvinar. (vi) Salience network; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the mediodorsal nucleus and the pulvinar. (vii) Dorsal attention network; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the pulvinar, and mediodorsal nucleus and higher thalamo-cortical connectivity in the anteroventral nucleus. (viii) Right and left executive network; patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the mediodorsal nucleus, pulvinar and in the lateral group. (ix) Left executive network: patients showed higher thalamo-cortical connectivity in the pulvinar. Notes. DMN, default mode network; L-EF, left executive network; R-EF, right executive network. The right side of the brain is on the right side of the image. All sub-regions of the thalamus are thresholded at FDR-corrected P = 0.05. Only FDR-corrected voxels are depicted in the Figure.

Group differences in thalamo-cortical networks

Figure 1 summarizes group differences in thalamo-cortical connectivity. Analyses revealed significant differences between patients and controls in thalamo-cortical connectivity in all resting-state networks, including the motor, auditory, visual, DMN, posterior DMN, salience, left/right executive and dorsal attention networks.

For the motor network, patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the mediodorsal nucleus and in nuclei from the anterior group, whereas higher thalamo-cortical connectivity was found in nuclei from the lateral group. For the visual network, patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the pulvinar, ventral lateral, ventral posterior, mediodorsal,and lateral posterior nuclei. For the auditory network, patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the lateral and medial nuclei, including the lateral dorsal and mediodorsal nuclei. For the DMN, patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the pulvinar, whereas higher thalamo-cortical connectivity was found in the mediodorsal nucleus. For the posterior DMN, patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the lateral nuclei and the pulvinar. For the salience network, patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the mediodorsal nucleus and the pulvinar. For the dorsal attention network, patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the pulvinar and mediodorsal nucleus, whereas higher thalamo-cortical connectivity was found in the anteroventral nucleus. For the right and left executive network, patients showed lower thalamo-cortical connectivity in the mediodorsal nucleus, pulvinar, and in the lateral group. For the left executive network, patients showed in addition higher thalamo-cortical connectivity in the pulvinar.

To further ensure that the observed group differences in thalamo-cortical connectivity strength were not originated by the implemented lesion masks, a further analysis was conducted. We applied the same lesion masks as for the AIS population to the images of the healthy controls. The ‘lesion masks’ for the healthy controls were exactly the same with respect to the total number of voxels and were exactly positioned at the same location as for the patients. A paired t-test was conducted to compare the thalamo-cortical connectivity between healthy controls and healthy controls with the implemented lesion masks. Analyses revealed no substantial differences in the thalamo-cortical connectivity strength (supplementary Fig. 2).

Lastly, as there were three patients in the sample with thalamic infarctions, we conducted a supplementary analysis investigating the thalamo-cortical networks without these three patients (N = 17 i.e. excluding the patients who have lesions in the thalamus). These results are shown in supplementary Fig. 2.

Thalamo-cortical connectivity and cognition

Figure 2 summarizes the association between thalamo-cortical connectivity and cognitive performance in patients. Multivariate linear regression (lesion size and age as covariates) revealed significant associations between cognitive performance and the strength of thalamo-cortical connectivity in the motor, auditory, visual, DMN, posterior DMN, salience, left/right executive, and dorsal attention network after paediatric stroke (Fig. 2; supplementary Table 2).

Cognition and thalamo-cortical networks. Association between thalamo-cortical connectivity and cognitive performance in paediatric patients after stroke. (A) Selective attention showed significant associations with connectivity for all resting networks; positive associations were found in the intralaminar, lateral dorsal, lateral posterior, and the mediodorsal nucleus; negative associations were found in the pulvinar and the anteroventral nucleus. (B) Inhibition showed significant associations with connectivity for all resting networks; positive associations were found in the mediodorsal, intralaminar, ventral lateral nucleus, and pulvinar; negative associations were found in the anteroventral nucleus, mediodorsal nucleus, and sub-nuclei of the lateral group. (C) Working memory showed significant associations with connectivity for all resting-state networks; positive associations were found in the mediodorsal, intralaminar, ventral lateral, and pulvinar; negative associations were found in the mediodorsal nucleus, intralaminar, and ventral lateral nucleus. Notes. WM, working memory; DMN, default mode network; L-EF, left executive network; R-EF, right executive network. The right side of the brain is on the right side of the image (including the data that was flipped). Illustrated significant clusters in different sub-nuclei of the thalamus result from the multivariate linear regression with lesion size and age as covariates. All sub-regions of the thalamus are thresholded at FDR-corrected P = 0.05. Only FDR-corrected voxels are depicted in the Figure.
Figure 2

Cognition and thalamo-cortical networks. Association between thalamo-cortical connectivity and cognitive performance in paediatric patients after stroke. (A) Selective attention showed significant associations with connectivity for all resting networks; positive associations were found in the intralaminar, lateral dorsal, lateral posterior, and the mediodorsal nucleus; negative associations were found in the pulvinar and the anteroventral nucleus. (B) Inhibition showed significant associations with connectivity for all resting networks; positive associations were found in the mediodorsal, intralaminar, ventral lateral nucleus, and pulvinar; negative associations were found in the anteroventral nucleus, mediodorsal nucleus, and sub-nuclei of the lateral group. (C) Working memory showed significant associations with connectivity for all resting-state networks; positive associations were found in the mediodorsal, intralaminar, ventral lateral, and pulvinar; negative associations were found in the mediodorsal nucleus, intralaminar, and ventral lateral nucleus. Notes. WM, working memory; DMN, default mode network; L-EF, left executive network; R-EF, right executive network. The right side of the brain is on the right side of the image (including the data that was flipped). Illustrated significant clusters in different sub-nuclei of the thalamus result from the multivariate linear regression with lesion size and age as covariates. All sub-regions of the thalamus are thresholded at FDR-corrected P = 0.05. Only FDR-corrected voxels are depicted in the Figure.

Multivariate linear regressions in the patient sample with selective attention as independent variable and thalamo-cortical connectivity as dependent variable (lesion size and age as covariates) revealed significant associations for all resting networks. Positive associations were found in the intralaminar, lateral dorsal, lateral posterior, and the mediodorsal nucleus; negative associations were found in the pulvinar and the anteroventral nucleus (Fig. 2; supplementary Table 2). Multivariate linear regressions with inhibition as independent variable and thalamo-cortical connectivity as dependent variable (with lesion size and age as covariates) revealed significant associations for all resting networks. Specifically, positive associations were found in the mediodorsal, intralaminar, ventral lateral nucleus, and pulvinar. Negative associations were found in the anteroventral nucleus, mediodorsal nucleus, and sub-nuclei of the lateral group (Fig. 2; supplementary Table 2). Multivariate linear regressions with working memory as independent variable and thalamo-cortical connectivity as a dependent variable (lesion size and age as covariates), revealed significant associations for all resting networks. Specifically, positive associations were found in the mediodorsal, intralaminar, ventral lateral, and pulvinar. Negative associations occurred in the mediodorsal nucleus, intralaminar, and ventral lateral nucleus (Fig. 2; supplementary Table 2). Cognitive flexibility showed no significant associations with thalamo-cortical connectivity. All reported results remained significant after FDR correction. Voxels and t values are listed in Table S2.

Discussion

The present cross-sectional study aimed to compare resting-state thalamo-cortical connectivity between patients after unilateral paediatric AIS and healthy controls. We further explored the relationship between thalamo-cortical connectivity and cognitive outcome in patients after paediatric stroke. Cognitive functions were significantly reduced in patients compared to healthy controls, supporting the findings of previous studies.62–64 According to our primary hypothesis, we found significantly weaker thalamo-cortical connectivity for the motor, auditory, visual, DMN, salience, left/right executive, and dorsal attention network in patients compared with controls. In additionally, we also found stronger thalamo-cortical connectivity in the DMN, motor, dorsal attention, and the left executive network in patients compared with controls. Stronger thalamo-cortical connectivity for patients compared with controls was however found in different thalamic nuclei, pointing to hypo- as well as hyperconnectivity in the patients sample in different parts of the thalamus. According to our secondary hypothesis, thalamo-cortical connectivity strength was positively associated with selective attention, inhibition and working memory. In accordance with the results of the group analyses, we also found negative associations between cognitive functions and thalamo-cortical connectivity strength. The significant relationship between thalamo-cortical connectivity and cognitive functions after paediatric stroke has never been described previously.

Group differences in thalamo-cortical networks

Nine resting-state networks were identified and were comparable to the networks that have previously been observed in rs-fMRI studies.19,22,26,27,61 Significant differences between patients and controls were found for all thalamo-cortical networks within all three major nuclear regions, as well as the pulvinar and the intralaminar nuclei. Furthermore, between-group differences were observed in the ipsilesional, as well as in the contralesional hemisphere, which supports the assumption that focal structural brain damage can lead to alterations in remote regions.33,41,65

Specifically, patients showed weaker thalamo-cortical connectivity in the motor, auditory, visual, DMN, salience, left/right executive, and dorsal attention network. Our finding of reduced functional connectivity strength in patients is in line with previous findings in paediatric37,38 and adult stroke populations.23,36,66–69 Reduced connectivity strength might be interpreted as sustained interruption of functional network efficiency. Interestingly, we also found stronger thalamo-cortical connectivity in other thalamic subdivisions in the DMN (medial nuclei), motor (lateral nuclei), dorsal attention (anterior nuclei), and the left executive network (posterior nuclei) in patients compared with controls. Stronger thalamo-cortical connectivity was however found in different thalamic nuclei. The finding of higher thalamo-cortical connectivity strength in patients than in controls may reflect a compensatory effect due to reorganizational processes occurring after brain damage.9 Thus, alterations in thalamo-cortical connectivity strength after paediatric stroke seem to be in both directions, with stronger as well as weaker thalamo-cortical connectivity, likely representing different patterns of functional connectivity after paediatric stroke. Our findings of increased and decreased functional thalamo-cortical connectivity strength thereby build on previous literature in stroke populations reporting increased and decreased resting-state functional connectivity on a cortical level.9,33,38,70

In addition, it has been shown previously that both cortical and subcortical areas, such as the thalamus, show alterations in connectivity strength after stroke affecting both the ipsi- and the contralesional hemisphere.33,41,71 One possible explanation derives from work describing the process of diaschisis,72,73 which showed that stroke can also induce changes in the function and physiology of brain regions distant from the site of anatomical damage. This concept is supported by studies on homogeneous patient groups (e.g. ischaemic strokes in basal ganglia or periventricular venous infarction), which demonstrated altered connectivity strength in structures remote from the lesion.41,65 Alterations of functional connectivity in the contralesional hemisphere seen in our study might therefore support theories of network organization of the brain.71,74

The fact that our patients were assessed in the chronic post-stroke phase (at least 2 years after AIS) highlights that there are persistent alterations in functional thalamo-cortical connectivity even years after the stroke. A growing number of studies indicate that stroke induces changes of functional connectivity within and between resting-state networks in the chronic phase29,65,75 and that these changes correlate with chronic impairment post-stroke.65,76 The question whether the motor networks is affected by a hemiparesis, was addressed in one of our previous studies.40 We investigated functional connectivity in paediatric patients after stroke with hemiparesis compared with patients with a good clinical outcome and healthy controls. Patients with hemiparesis showed lower interhemispheric connectivity strength in the motor network compared to patients with good clinical outcome and controls.40

To further ensure that the observed group differences in thalamo-cortical connectivity strength were not originated by the implemented lesion masks, a further analysis with the same lesion masks was conducted. Analyses investigating the effect of the implemented lesion masks revealed no substantial differences in the thalamo-cortical connectivity strength (Supplementary Fig. 2). If the implementation of the lesion mask were influencing the strength of the thalamo-cortical connectivity, then differences in the thalamo-cortical connectivity should have been observed. Finally, as there were three patients in the sample with thalamic infarctions, we conducted a supplementary analysis investigating the thalamo-cortical networks without these three patients. Analyses revealed that there were only minimal changes in the t-test comparing patients and controls. Thus, these three patients did not substantially affect the results presented in the study.

Thalamo-cortical connectivity and cognition

Selective attention, inhibition and working memory was positively associated with functional thalamo-cortical connectivity strength in networks that have previously been related to cognitive processing (posterior DMN, salience, dorsal attention, and executive network).77 Further, our analyses also revealed significant positive associations between thalamo-cortical connectivity and cognitive functions in networks involved in visual, auditory, and motor-related processes in our patient sample. This might be because most cognitive functions arise not solely from a particular brain area but from networks spanning multiple distributed regions.78,79 Furthermore, the cognitive tasks require not only specific cognitive skills but also sensory-related functions such as visual and auditory related processing.80,81 Multiple processes underlie the performance of a cognitive task and no task is a pure measure of a single cognitive process.80,81 We also found negative associations between cognitive functions and thalamo-cortical connectivity for several networks. These associations were, however, in different parts of the thalamus, pointing towards a specialization of certain thalamic sub-regions.

The cancellation task used to measure selective attention is a visuo-spatial search task. Its performance was associated with multiple thalamo-cortical networks, with a predominant pattern of significant correlations for the visual, salience, and executive networks. For the visual network, positive associations with selective attention were found in the pulvinar, meaning that the better the selective attention, the higher the connectivity strength in this thalamic nucleus. The pulvinar has widespread reciprocal connectivity to different cortical areas, such as occipital, temporal, parietal and frontal regions. The pulvinar has previously been related to visuo-spatial attention processing.12,82 For the salience and executive network, positive associations with selective attention were found in the anterior nuclei and the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus, which are known to influence cognitive processes, including attention and executive functions.11,83,84 In addition, significant positive associations were observed with the auditory and motor-related network, possibly indicating that sensory-related thalamo-cortical networks may also be relevant for selective attention.

Inhibition is a core dimension of executive functions46 and was measured with colour–word interference tasks (comparable with a Stroop task). Performance of this task was associated with multiple thalamo-cortical networks, with a predominant pattern of positive correlations for the salience, dorsal attention and left and right executive networks (Fig. 2, supplementary Table 2). Specifically, the analyses revealed that the better the inhibition, the stronger the thalamo-cortical connectivity in these networks. Significant positive associations with inhibition were also observed for the visual, auditory, and motor-related networks. This might indicate that sensory-related thalamo-cortical networks could also be relevant for inhibition. In line with this proposition, inhibitory performance has been attributed to spatially distributed functional networks.79 Higher-order cognitive capacities have been shown to depend on complex large-scale brain systems.85

Working memory is another core dimension of executive function46 and was measured with a visuo-spatial location task. Working memory performance was associated with multiple thalamo-cortical networks, with a predominant pattern of positive correlations for the left and right executive networks (Fig. 2, supplementary Table 2). Visuo-spatial working memory function is based on a complex network of frontal, parietal, occipital and temporal areas.86 Significant positive associations were also observed for the visual, auditory and motor-related networks, indicating that sensory-related thalamo-cortical networks also seem to be relevant for visuo-spatial working memory.

Finally, the question arises why cognitive flexibility was not significantly associated with thalamo-cortical connectivity. First, from a statistical point of view, this correlation did not survive FDR correction but showed, however, non-corrected significant correlations. Different assumptions might be possible as to why there were only weak correlations between thalamo-cortical connectivity and cognitive flexibility. First, as there was only a trend for cognitive flexibility to be lower in patients after stroke, this cognitive domain seems to be less affected compared the other cognitive domains. Accordingly, studies with different clinical samples have shown that cognitive flexibility (assessed with the colour–word inference test) was less affected than inhibition.87–89 Since cognitive flexibility is the fourth and last condition of the colour–word inference test, patients might benefit from some familiarization or even learning effect occurring throughout the two conditions, leading to better cognitive flexibility than inhibition.90

Our findings therefore support the assumption, that higher-order cognitive functions after childhood AIS depend on the integrated processing of multiple large-scale functional brain networks,85 with the thalamus functioning as an integrative hub for cortical networks.10 Post-stroke cognitive outcome in our sample may reflect altered functional connections across multiple networks.

Limitations

The findings of the present study should be viewed in the light of some limitations. First, our sample size was relatively small. Therefore, further research is needed to replicate our findings in a larger cohort and confirm that brain connectivity in the thalamo-cortical network is related to long-term cognitive outcome. Second, it is difficult to precisely locate each thalamic nucleus using a standardized template since thalamic nuclei differ in size and locations and there is rather low spatial resolution of the functional images. Further, the thalamus atlas was constructed from histological data on adults60 and afterwards reconstructed in the MNI space.59 Therefore, a potential mismatch is expected. We therefore focused mainly on the largest of the 40 nuclei present in the Morel atlas, without differentiating finer subdivisions of the different nuclei. Moreover, in patients with large lesions (e.g. involving cortical and subcortical brain regions), it may be in general challenging to define the thalamus mask due to midline shift. However, we did not face difficulties in defining the thalamus mask for these patients during coregistration. We conducted a supplementary analysis investigating the thalamo-cortical networks excluding the patients who had lesions in the thalamus (N = 17). This analysis revealed no substantial differences in the thalamo-cortical connectivity strength (Supplementary Fig. 3). Finally, cross-sectional study designs provide information at a given time point in patients after stroke. However, as functional networks and cognitive outcomes likely change over time during development, longitudinal study designs are needed to gain a deeper insight into the development of thalamo-cortical networks and their possible relation to cognitive rehabilitation in children and adolescents.

Conclusion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first set of results showing a relationship between thalamo-cortical functional connectivity and cognitive functions (selective attention, working memory, and inhibition) after childhood stroke. Our data provide evidence that the interaction between different sub-nuclei of the thalamus and multiple cortical networks is crucial for post-stroke cognitive functions. Furthermore, the data support the idea that large-scale networks such as the thalamo-cortical system may help to explain the variability in cognitive outcomes after childhood AIS. Future investigations of recovery mechanisms after brain injury and the development of novel interventions and therapies should consider the network perspective of brain functions.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Brain Communications online.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank all children and adolescents who participated and their parents. Furthermore, we thank everybody who was involved in data acquisition, namely Maria Regenyi, Salome Kornfeld, Juan Delgado-Rodriguez, and Sandeep Kamal. Special thanks go to all co-workers at the Swiss Neuropaediatric Stroke Registry, namely Andrea Capone Mori (Aarau), Alexandre Datta (Basel), Joël Fluss (Geneva), Annette Hackenberg (Zurich), Elmar Keller (Chur), Oliver Maier (St. Gallen), Claudia Poloni (Lausanne and Sion), Barbara Goeggel Simonetti (Bellinzona), Regula Schmid (Winterthur), and Thomas Schmitt-Mechelke (Lucerne). Furthermore, we thank Susan Kaplan for editing the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (32003B_146894/1) and funds from the Anna Mueller Grocholski Foundation (Switzerland), Schweizerische Stiftung für das Cerebral Gelähmte Kind (Switzerland), and the Vinetum Foundation (Switzerland).

Competing interests

The authors report no competing interests.

References

1

Greenham
M
,
Gordon
A
,
Anderson
V
,
Mackay
MT
.
Outcome in childhood stroke
.
Stroke
.
2016
;
47
:
1159
1164
.

2

Kolk
A
,
Ennok
M
,
Laugesaar
R
,
Kaldoja
ML
,
Talvik
T
.
Long-term cognitive outcomes after pediatric stroke
.
Pediatr Neurol
.
2011
;
44
:
101
109
.

3

Steinlin
M
.
A clinical approach to arterial ischemic childhood stroke: Increasing knowledge over the last decade
.
Neuropediatrics
.
2012
;
43
:
1
9
.

4

Caspar-Teuscher
M
,
Studer
M
,
Regenyi
M
,
Steinlin
M
,
Grunt
S
,
Swiss Neuropediatric Stroke Registry Group
.
Health related quality of life and manual ability;5, years after neonatal ischemic stroke
.
Eur J Paediatr Neurol
.
23
:
716
722
.

5

Kornfeld
S
,
Studer
M
,
Winkelbeiner
S
,
Regényi
M
,
Boltshauser
E
,
Steinlin
M
.
Quality of life after paediatric ischaemic stroke
.
Dev Med Child Neurol
.
2017
;
59
:
45
51
.

6

Wiedemann
A
,
Pastore-Wapp
M
,
Slavova
N
, et al.
Impact of stroke volume on motor outcome in neonatal arterial ischemic stroke
.
Eur J Paediatr Neurol
.
2020
;
25
:
97
105
.

7

Warren
DE
,
Power
JD
,
Bruss
J
, et al.
Network measures predict neuropsychological outcome after brain injury
.
Proc Nat Acad Sci USA
.
2014
;
111
:
14247
14252
.

8

Adhikari
MH
,
Raja Beharelle
A
,
Griffa
A
, et al.
Computational modeling of resting-state activity demonstrates markers of normalcy in children with prenatal or perinatal stroke
.
J Neurosci
.
2015
;
35
:
8914
8924
.

9

Ilves
N
,
Ilves
P
,
Laugesaar
R
, et al.
Resting-state functional connectivity and cognitive impairment in children with perinatal stroke
.
Neural Plast
.
2016
;
2016
:
2306406
.

10

Hwang
K
,
Bertolero
MA
,
Liu
WB
,
D’Esposito
M
.
The human thalamus is an integrative hub for functional brain networks
.
J Neurosci
.
2017
;
37
:
5594
5607
.

11

Saalmann
YB
.
Intralaminar and medial thalamic influence on cortical synchrony, information transmission and cognition
.
Front Syst Neurosci
.
2014
;
8
:
83
.

12

Halassa
MM
,
Kastner
S
.
Thalamic functions in distributed cognitive control
.
Nat Neurosci
.
2017
;
20
:
1669
1679
.

13

Nakajima
M
,
Halassa
MM
.
Thalamic control of functional cortical connectivity
.
Curr Opin Neurobiol
.
2017
;
44
:
127
131
.

14

Fama
R
,
Sullivan
EV
.
Thalamic structures and associated cognitive functions: Relations with age and aging
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2015
;
54
:
29
37
.

15

Anton-Bolanos
N
,
Espinosa
A
,
Lopez-Bendito
G
.
Developmental interactions between thalamus and cortex: A true love reciprocal story
.
Curr Opin Neurobiol
.
2018
;
52
:
33
41
.

16

Lopez-Bendito
G
.
Development of the thalamocortical interactions: Past, present and future
.
Neuroscience
.
2018
;
385
:
67
74
.

17

Ferguson
BR
,
Gao
WJ
.
Thalamic control of cognition and social behavior via regulation of gamma-aminobutyric acidergic signaling and excitation/inhibition balance in the medial prefrontal cortex
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2018
;
83
:
657
669
.

18

Mitchell
AS
,
Sherman
SM
,
Sommer
MA
,
Mair
RG
,
Vertes
RP
,
Chudasama
Y
.
Advances in understanding mechanisms of thalamic relays in cognition and behavior
.
J Neurosci
.
2014
;
34
:
15340
15346
.

19

Steiner
L
,
Federspiel
A
,
Slavova
N
, et al.
Functional topography of the thalamo-cortical system during development and its relation to cognition
.
NeuroImage
.
2020
;
223
:
117361
.

20

Bordes
S
,
Werner
C
,
Mathkour
M
, et al.
Arterial supply of the thalamus: A comprehensive review
.
World Neurosurg
.
2020
;
137
:
310
318
.

21

Zhang
D
,
Snyder
AZ
,
Fox
MD
,
Sansbury
MW
,
Shimony
JS
,
Raichle
ME
.
Intrinsic functional relations between human cerebral cortex and thalamus
.
J Neurophysiol
.
2008
;
100
:
1740
1748
.

22

Yuan
R
,
Di
X
,
Taylor
PA
,
Gohel
S
,
Tsai
YH
,
Biswal
BB
.
Functional topography of the thalamocortical system in human
.
Brain Struct Funct
.
2016
;
221
:
1971
1984
.

23

Kim
DJ
,
Park
B
,
Park
HJ
.
Functional connectivity-based identification of subdivisions of the basal ganglia and thalamus using multilevel independent component analysis of resting state fMRI
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2013
;
34
:
1371
85
.

24

Van Dijk
KR
,
Hedden
T
,
Venkataraman
A
,
Evans
KC
,
Lazar
SW
,
Buckner
RL
.
Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: Theory, properties, and optimization
.
J Neurophysiol
.
2010
;
103
:
297
321
.

25

Biswal
B
,
Yetkin
FZ
,
Haughton
VM
,
Hyde
JS
.
Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI
.
Magn Reson Med
.
1995
;
34
:
537
541
.

26

Fox
MD
,
Raichle
ME
.
Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2007
;
8
:
700
711
.

27

Thomason
ME
,
Dennis
EL
,
Joshi
AA
, et al.
Resting-state fMRI can reliably map neural networks in children
.
NeuroImage
.
2011
;
55
:
165
175
.

28

Uddin
LQ
,
Supekar
K
,
Menon
V
.
Typical and atypical development of functional human brain networks: Insights from resting-state FMRI
.
Front Syst Neurosci
.
2010
;
4
:
21
.

29

Ovadia-Caro
S
,
Villringer
K
,
Fiebach
J
, et al.
Longitudinal effects of lesions on functional networks after stroke
.
J Cereb Blood Flow Metab
.
2013
;
33
:
1279
1285
.

30

Siegel
JS
,
Ramsey
LE
,
Snyder
AZ
, et al.
Disruptions of network connectivity predict impairment in multiple behavioral domains after stroke
.
Proc Nat Acad Sci USA
.
2016
;
113
:
E4367
E4376
.

31

Warren
JE
,
Crinion
JT
,
Lambon Ralph
MA
,
Wise
RJ
.
Anterior temporal lobe connectivity correlates with functional outcome after aphasic stroke
.
Brain
.
2009
;
132
:
3428
3442
.

32

Klingbeil
J
,
Wawrzyniak
M
,
Stockert
A
,
Saur
D
.
Resting-state functional connectivity: An emerging method for the study of language networks in post-stroke aphasia
.
Brain Cogn
.
2019
;
131
:
22
33
.

33

Baldassarre
A
,
Ramsey
LE
,
Siegel
JS
,
Shulman
GL
,
Corbetta
M
.
Brain connectivity and neurological disorders after stroke
.
Curr Opin Neurol
.
2016
;
29
:
706
713
.

34

van Meer
MP
,
van der Marel
K
,
Wang
K
, et al.
Recovery of sensorimotor function after experimental stroke correlates with restoration of resting-state interhemispheric functional connectivity
.
J Neurosci
.
2010
;
30
:
3964
3972
.

35

Liu
J
,
Qin
W
,
Zhang
J
,
Zhang
X
,
Yu
C
.
Enhanced interhemispheric functional connectivity compensates for anatomical connection damages in subcortical stroke
.
Stroke
.
2015
;
46
:
1045
1051
.

36

Park
CH
,
Chang
WH
,
Ohn
SH
, et al.
Longitudinal changes of resting-state functional connectivity during motor recovery after stroke
.
Stroke
.
2011
;
42
:
1357
1362
.

37

Kornfeld
S
,
Yuan
R
,
Biswal
BB
, et al.
Resting-state connectivity and executive functions after pediatric arterial ischemic stroke
.
Neuroimage Clin
.
2018
;
17
:
359
367
.

38

Saunders
J
,
Carlson
HL
,
Cortese
F
,
Goodyear
BG
,
Kirton
A
.
Imaging functional motor connectivity in hemiparetic children with perinatal stroke
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2018
;
40
:
1632
1642
.

39

Dick
AS
,
Raja Beharelle
A
,
Solodkin
A
,
Small
SL
.
Interhemispheric functional connectivity following prenatal or perinatal brain injury predicts receptive language outcome
.
J Neurosci
.
2013
;
33
:
5612
5625
.

40

Steiner
L
,
Homan
S
,
Everts
R
, et al.
Functional connectivity and upper limb function in patients after pediatric arterial ischemic stroke with contralateral corticospinal tract wiring
.
Sci Rep
.
2021
;
11
:
5490
.

41

Carlson
HL
,
Craig
BT
,
Hilderley
AJ
, et al.
Structural and functional connectivity of motor circuits after perinatal stroke: A machine learning study
.
Neuroimage Clin
.
2020
;
28
:
102508
.

42

Craig
BT
,
Carlson
HL
,
Kirton
A
.
Thalamic diaschisis following perinatal stroke is associated with clinical disability
.
Neuroimage Clin
.
2019
;
21
:
101660
.

43

Kornfeld
S
,
Delgado Rodriguez
JA
,
Everts
R
, et al.
Cortical reorganisation of cerebral networks after childhood stroke: Impact on outcome
.
BMC Neurol
.
2015
;
15
:
90
.

44

Steinlin
M
,
Pfister
I
,
Pavlovic
J
, et al.
The first three years of the Swiss neuropaediatric stroke registry (SNPSR): A population-based study of incidence, symptoms and risk factors
.
Neuropediatrics
.
2005
;
36
:
90
97
.

45

Ballester
BR
,
Maier
M
,
Duff
A
, et al.
A critical time window for recovery extends beyond one-year post-stroke
.
J Neurophysiol
.
2019
;
122
:
350
357
.

46

Buttelmann
F
,
Karbach
J
.
Development and plasticity of cognitive flexibility in early and middle childhood
.
Front Psychol
.
2017
;
8
:
1040
.

47

Lepach
AC
,
Gienger
C
,
Petermann
F
.
Neuropsychological findings on memory and learning disabilities in children using the BASIC-MLT
.
Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother
.
2008
;
36
:
389
398
.
quiz 399-400. Neuropsychologische Befunde zu Merk- und Lernstorungen bei Kindern anhand des BASIC-MLT
.

48

Delis
DC
,
Kramer
JH
,
Kaplan
E
,
Holdnack
J
.
Reliability and validity of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: An update
.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc
.
2004
;
10
:
301
303
.

49

Everts
R
,
Pavlovic
J
,
Kaufmann
F
, et al.
Cognitive functioning, behavior, and quality of life after stroke in childhood
.
Child Neuropsychol
.
2008
;
14
:
323
338
.

50

Griffis
JC
,
Allendorfer
JB
,
Szaflarski
JP
.
Voxel-based Gaussian naïve Bayes classification of ischemic stroke lesions in individual T1-weighted MRI scans
.
J Neurosci Methods
.
2016
;
257
:
97
108
.

51

Ito
KL
,
Kim
H
,
Liew
SL
.
A comparison of automated lesion segmentation approaches for chronic stroke T1-weighted MRI data
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2019
;
40
:
4669
4685
.

52

Smith
SM
,
Jenkinson
M
,
Woolrich
MW
, et al.
Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL
.
Neuroimage
.
2004
;
23
:
S208
S219
.

53

Woolrich
MW
,
Jbabdi
S
,
Patenaude
B
, et al.
Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL
.
Neuroimage
.
2009
;
45
:
S173
S186
.

54

Jenkinson
M
,
Beckmann
CF
,
Behrens
TE
,
Woolrich
MW
,
Smith
SM
.
Fsl
.
Neuroimage
.
2012
;
62
:
782
790
.

55

Power
JD
,
Mitra
A
,
Laumann
TO
,
Snyder
AZ
,
Schlaggar
BL
,
Petersen
SE
.
Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI
.
Neuroimage
.
2014
;
84
:
320
341
.

56

Power
JD
,
Schlaggar
BL
,
Petersen
SE
.
Recent progress and outstanding issues in motion correction in resting state fMRI
.
NeuroImage
.
2015
;
105
:
536
551
.

57

Fox
MD
,
Zhang
D
,
Snyder
AZ
,
Raichle
ME
.
The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks
.
J Neurophysiol
.
2009
;
101
:
3270
3283
.

58

Murphy
K
,
Birn
RM
,
Handwerker
DA
,
Jones
TB
,
Bandettini
PA
.
The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: Are anti-correlated networks introduced?
Neuroimage
.
2009
;
44
:
893
905
.

59

Krauth
A
,
Blanc
R
,
Poveda
A
,
Jeanmonod
D
,
Morel
A
,
Szekely
G
.
A mean three-dimensional atlas of the human thalamus: Generation from multiple histological data
.
Neuroimage
.
2010
;
49
:
2053
2062
.

60

Morel
A
,
Magnin
M
,
Jeanmonod
D
.
Multiarchitectonic and stereotactic atlas of the human thalamus
.
J Comp Neurol
.
1997
;
387
:
588
630
.

61

Alcauter
S
,
Lin
W
,
Smith
JK
, et al.
Development of thalamocortical connectivity during infancy and its cognitive correlations
.
J Neurosci
.
2014
;
34
:
9067
9075
.

62

Long
B
,
Spencer-Smith
MM
,
Jacobs
R
, et al.
Executive function following child stroke: The impact of lesion location
.
J Child Neurol
.
2011
;
26
:
279
287
.

63

O’Keeffe
F
,
Liegeois
F
,
Eve
M
,
Ganesan
V
,
King
J
,
Murphy
T
.
Neuropsychological and neurobehavioral outcome following childhood arterial ischemic stroke: Attention deficits, emotional dysregulation, and executive dysfunction
.
Child Neuropsychol
.
2014
;
20
:
557
582
.

64

Studer
M
,
Boltshauser
E
,
Capone Mori
A
, et al.
Factors affecting cognitive outcome in early pediatric stroke
.
Neurology
.
2014
;
82
:
784
92
.

65

He
BJ
,
Snyder
AZ
,
Vincent
JL
,
Epstein
A
,
Shulman
GL
,
Corbetta
M
.
Breakdown of functional connectivity in frontoparietal networks underlies behavioral deficits in spatial neglect
.
Neuron
.
2007
;
53
:
905
918
.

66

Thiel
A
,
Vahdat
S
.
Structural and resting-state brain connectivity of motor networks after stroke
.
Stroke
.
2015
;
46
:
296
301
.

67

Golestani
AM
,
Tymchuk
S
,
Demchuk
A
,
Goodyear
BG
,
Group
V-S
.
Longitudinal evaluation of resting-state FMRI after acute stroke with hemiparesis
.
Neurorehabil Neural Repair
.
2013
;
27
:
153
163
.

68

Liu
H
,
Tian
T
,
Qin
W
,
Li
K
,
Yu
C
.
Contrasting evolutionary patterns of functional connectivity in sensorimotor and cognitive regions after stroke
.
Front Behav Neurosci
.
2016
;
10
:
72
.

69

Rehme
AK
,
Grefkes
C
.
Cerebral network disorders after stroke: Evidence from imaging-based connectivity analyses of active and resting brain states in humans
.
J Physiol
.
2013
;
591
:
17
31
.

70

Boyd
LA
,
Hayward
KS
,
Ward
NS
, et al.
Biomarkers of stroke recovery: Consensus-based core recommendations from the stroke recovery and rehabilitation roundtable
.
Int J Stroke
.
2017
;
12
:
480
493
.

71

Corbetta
M
.
Functional connectivity and neurological recovery
.
Dev Psychobiol
.
2012
;
54
:
239
253
.

72

Carrera
E
,
Tononi
G
.
Diaschisis: Past, present, future
.
Brain
.
2014
;
137
(Pt
9
):
2408
2422
.

73

Slavova
N
,
Shojai
MP
,
Everts
R
,
Wiest
R
,
Steinlin
M
,
Grunt
S
.
Is asymmetry of the pons associated with hand function and manual ability after arterial ischemic stroke in children?
Neuropediatrics
.
2019
;
50
:
138
145
.

74

Fornito
A
,
Zalesky
A
,
Breakspear
M
.
The connectomics of brain disorders
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2015
;
16
:
159
172
.

75

Wang
C
,
Qin
W
,
Zhang
J
, et al.
Altered functional organization within and between resting-state networks in chronic subcortical infarction
.
J Cereb Blood Flow Metab
.
2014
;
34
:
597
605
.

76

Ramsey
LE
,
Siegel
JS
,
Baldassarre
A
, et al.
Normalization of network connectivity in hemispatial neglect recovery
.
Ann Neurol
.
2016
;
80
:
127
141
.

77

Smith
SM
,
Fox
PT
,
Miller
KL
, et al.
Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest
.
Proc Nat Acad Sci USA
.
2009
;
106
:
13040
13045
.

78

Petersen
SE
,
Sporns
O
.
Brain networks and cognitive architectures
.
Neuron
.
2015
;
88
:
207
219
.

79

Erika-Florence
M
,
Leech
R
,
Hampshire
A
.
A functional network perspective on response inhibition and attentional control
.
Nat Commun
.
2014
;
5
:
4073
.

80

Schumacher
R
,
Halai
AD
,
Lambon Ralph
MA
.
Assessing and mapping language, attention and executive multidimensional deficits in stroke aphasia
.
Brain
.
2019
;
142
:
3202
3216
.

81

Anderson
V
,
Spencer-Smith
M
,
Wood
A
.
Do children really recover better? Neurobehavioural plasticity after early brain insult
.
Brain
.
2011
;
134
(Pt
8
):
2197
2221
.

82

Wilke
M
,
Turchi
J
,
Smith
K
,
Mishkin
M
,
Leopold
DA
.
Pulvinar inactivation disrupts selection of movement plans
.
J Neurosci
.
2010
;
30
:
8650
8659
.

83

Golden
EC
,
Graff-Radford
J
,
Jones
J
,
Benarroch
EE
.
Mediodorsal nucleus and its multiple cognitive functions
.
Am Acad Neurol
.
2016
;
87
:
2161
2168
.

84

Watanabe
Y
,
Funahashi
S
.
Thalamic mediodorsal nucleus and working memory
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2012
;
36
:
134
142
.

85

Murphy
AC
,
Bertolero
MA
,
Papadopoulos
L
,
Lydon-Staley
DM
,
Bassett
DS
.
Multimodal network dynamics underpinning working memory
.
Nat Commun
.
2020
;
11
:
3035
.

86

Palva
JM
,
Monto
S
,
Kulashekhar
S
,
Palva
S
.
Neuronal synchrony reveals working memory networks and predicts individual memory capacity
.
Proc Nat Acad Sci USA
.
2010
;
107
:
7580
7585
.

87

Everts
R
,
Schöne
CG
,
Mürner-Lavanchy
I
,
Steinlin
M
.
Development of executive functions from childhood to adolescence in very preterm-born individuals - A longitudinal study
.
Early Hum Dev
.
2019
;
129
:
45
51
.

88

Ritter
BC
,
Nelle
M
,
Perrig
W
,
Steinlin
M
,
Everts
R
.
Executive functions of children born very preterm–deficit or delay?
Eur J Pediatr
.
2013
;
172
:
473
483
.

89

Wehrle
FM
,
Stöckli
A
,
Disselhoff
V
, et al.
Effects of correcting for prematurity on executive function scores of children born very preterm at school age
.
J Pediatri
.
2021
;
238
:
145
152.e2
.

90

Lippa
SM
,
Davis
RN
.
Inhibition/switching is not necessarily harder than inhibition: An analysis of the D-KEFS color-word interference test
.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol
.
2010
;
25
:
146
152
.

Abbreviations

     
  • AIS =

    arterial ischaemic stroke

  •  
  • BOLD =

    blood-oxygen-level-dependent

  •  
  • DMN =

    default mode network

  •  
  • FDR =

    false discovery rate

  •  
  • FD =

    framewise displacement

  •  
  • fMRI =

    functional magnetic resonance imaging

  •  
  • IC =

    independent component

  •  
  • ICAs =

    independent component analyses

  •  
  • IQ =

    intelligence quotient

  •  
  • MNI =

    Montreal Neurological Institute

  •  
  • rs-fMRI =

    resting-state functional MRI

  •  
  • SPM12 =

    statistical parametric mapping

Author notes

*

Leonie Steiner and Andrea Federspiel contributed equally to this work.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Supplementary data